This chapter provides an overview of proficiency in literacy, numeracy and adaptive problem solving among adults between 16 and 65 years old. It compares average proficiency across countries and economies, and examines how these skills are distributed. It also describes how adults with different socio-demographic characteristics (age, gender, educational attainment, immigrant background and parental education) differ in their skills proficiency.
Do Adults Have the Skills They Need to Thrive in a Changing World?

2. Literacy, numeracy and adaptive problem solving among adults in 2023
Copy link to 2. Literacy, numeracy and adaptive problem solving among adults in 2023Abstract
In Brief
Copy link to In BriefAcross the OECD countries and economies that participated in the 2023 Survey of Adult Skills, the average scores for adults aged 16-65 are 260 points in literacy, 263 points in numeracy and 251 points in adaptive problem solving, on scales ranging from 0 to 500 points. Adults in Finland achieved the highest scores in literacy (296 points) and numeracy (294 points), as well as in adaptive problem solving (276 points, the same score achieved by adults in Japan).
Large shares of the adult population scored at the two lowest levels of the proficiency scales: 26% in literacy, 25% in numeracy and 29% in adaptive problem solving, on average across OECD countries and economies. In Chile, 44% of adults scored at the two lowest levels in all three skill domains, compared to only 7% in Japan.
On average, 55-65 year-olds display lower proficiency than younger adults in all the assessed domains. The best results were achieved by 25-34 year-olds, followed by 16-24 year-olds.
Higher levels of educational attainment are associated with greater proficiency in literacy, numeracy and adaptive problem solving. On average, adults with tertiary education scored 33 points higher in literacy than those with upper secondary education, who in turn scored 43 points higher than those without upper secondary education.
Gender gaps in proficiency are generally small, especially in literacy and adaptive problem solving. Women displayed higher average proficiency than men in literacy (a difference of 3 points), while men scored higher in numeracy (10 points) and adaptive problem solving (2 points).
Native-born adults of native-born parents displayed much higher proficiency in all domains than foreign-born adults of foreign-born parents (differences of 44 points in literacy, 38 points in numeracy and 32 points in adaptive problem solving). Much of these gaps are explained by the different socio-demographic characteristics of these two groups.
Adults who grew up in advantaged socio-economic conditions displayed greater proficiency in all skill domains. Having at least one tertiary-educated parent is associated with an advantage of 50 points in literacy, 49 points in numeracy and 42 points in adaptive problem solving, when compared to adults with no parent who has attained upper secondary education.
Introduction
Copy link to IntroductionThe 2023 Survey of Adult Skills provides a snapshot of adults’ proficiency in literacy, numeracy and adaptive problem solving. These are foundation skills that enable individuals to engage and function effectively across a broad spectrum of everyday situations and to perform the tasks required by their various social roles, in everyday life as well as in their jobs. Command of these skills enables adults to:
achieve personal goals, cultivate their knowledge and potential, and actively engage in society
navigate and handle the mathematical challenges encountered in a range of situations in adult life
use cognitive and metacognitive processes to define problems, seek relevant information and implement solutions across diverse information environments and contexts.
As literacy, numeracy and adaptive problem solving are linked to several important economic and social outcomes for both individuals and countries (see Chapter 4), raising skills proficiency of all adults is crucial for individual and collective prosperity. The skills adults possess are determined by the learning opportunities available throughout their lives. These opportunities start during childhood and youth and continue into adulthood and old age. Therefore, policies must strive to give all citizens the same opportunities to develop and reach their full potential by eliminating the barriers to learning related to social circumstances outside of individuals’ control (OECD, 2021[1]; 2019[2]; 2018[3]).
Despite increasing and continuing efforts to ensure equal educational opportunities for all, no country has achieved a level playing field for everyone, although some succeed better than others. Disparities in access to resources persist among different socio-demographic groups, which are reflected in observed differences in proficiency between adults with low and high levels of education, between men and women, between immigrants and non-immigrants, and between adults raised in families with different economic and cultural resources.
To achieve equal opportunities, well-targeted policies in various areas continue to play a key role. For these policies to be effective, policy makers should carefully assess the challenges and barriers some population groups face. Policies should also be adaptable and responsive to evolving trends and crises, which can both exacerbate inequalities and potentially offer new avenues to achieve fair and equal conditions.
This chapter examines adults’ skills proficiency as assessed in the 2023 Survey of Adult Skills. Its first section reports on adults’ average proficiency in literacy, numeracy and adaptive problem solving in each participating country and economy, compared to adults in other countries and economies as well as to the OECD average. This section also presents the distribution of adults across proficiency levels in all domains. Finally, it examines aggregate inequalities, by considering the inter-decile range. This refers to the gap that separates the highest-performing and lowest-performing 10% of adults within each country and economy. Its second section looks at inequalities with respect to adults’ socio-demographic characteristics. It examines the average proficiency in literacy, numeracy and adaptive problem solving of adults in different socio-demographic groups, which are defined in terms of age, education, gender, immigrant background and parental education.
How did adults perform in the 2023 Survey of Adult Skills?
Copy link to How did adults perform in the 2023 Survey of Adult Skills?Average proficiency across countries
The average literacy proficiency of adults in OECD countries and economies participating in the 2023 Survey of Adult Skills is 260 points with a standard deviation of 55 points (Table 2.1). Adults in Finland scored, on average, significantly higher (296 points) than the averages for all other participating countries and economies. Adults in Japan (289 points), Sweden (284 points) and Norway (281 points) also achieved average scores above 280 points. In nine other countries and economies, adults performed significantly above the OECD average, ranging from an average of 279 points in the Netherlands to 263 points in Ireland. The results in Czechia and New Zealand (both 260 points) and the United States (258 points) were not statistically different from the OECD average. In contrast, adults in 15 countries scored significantly below the OECD average, with average scores ranging from 255 points in France and Singapore to 218 points in Chile.
The average numeracy proficiency among participating OECD countries and economies is 263 points with a standard deviation of 58 points (Table 2.2). Adults in Finland scored significantly higher (294 points on average) than those from all other participating countries and economies. In five other countries, average scores exceeded 280 points: Japan (291 points), Sweden and Norway (both 285 points), the Netherlands (284 points), and Estonia (281). Nine more countries and economies scored significantly above the OECD average, ranging from an average of 279 points in the Flemish Region (Belgium) and Denmark to 267 points in Czechia and Austria while the average scores in Latvia (263 points) and the Slovak Republic (261 points) were not statistically different from the OECD average. Fourteen countries scored significantly below the OECD average, ranging from 260 points in Ireland to 214 points in Chile.
Across OECD countries and economies, adults scored on average 251 points in adaptive problem solving with a standard deviation of 47 points (Table 2.3). Adults in Finland and Japan scored significantly higher (276 points on average in both countries) than those from all other participating countries and economies, followed by Sweden (273 points) and Norway (271 points). Another nine countries and economies scored above the OECD average, ranging from the Netherlands (average of 265 points) to Austria (253 points). Average scores in Singapore (252 points), Czechia (250 points), and New Zealand (249 points) were not statistically different from the OECD average. In contrast, 15 countries and economies scored below the OECD average, ranging from 249 points in Ireland and 248 points in France to 218 points in Chile.
Table 2.1. Comparison of countries and economies based on average proficiency in literacy
Copy link to Table 2.1. Comparison of countries and economies based on average proficiency in literacy
Mean score |
Comparison country/economy |
Countries and economies whose mean score is not statistically significantly different from the comparison country's/economy's score |
296 |
Finland |
|
289 |
Japan |
|
284 |
Sweden |
|
281 |
Norway |
Netherlands |
279 |
Netherlands |
Norway |
276 |
Estonia |
Flemish Region (BE) |
275 |
Flemish Region (BE) |
Denmark, Estonia |
273 |
Denmark |
Flemish Region (BE), Canada, England (UK) |
272 |
England (UK) |
Canada, Denmark |
271 |
Canada |
Denmark, England (UK) |
266 |
Switzerland |
Germany |
266 |
Germany |
Switzerland |
263 |
Ireland |
Czechia, New Zealand |
260 |
Czechia |
Ireland, New Zealand, United States |
260 |
OECD average |
Czechia, New Zealand, United States |
260 |
New Zealand |
Czechia, Ireland, United States |
258 |
United States |
Czechia, Croatia, New Zealand |
255 |
France |
Austria, Croatia, Singapore, Slovak Republic |
255 |
Singapore |
Austria, France, Croatia, Slovak Republic |
254 |
Austria |
France, Croatia, Singapore, Slovak Republic |
254 |
Croatia |
Austria, France, Singapore, Slovak Republic, United States |
254 |
Slovak Republic |
Austria, France, Croatia, Singapore |
249 |
Korea |
Spain, Hungary, Latvia |
248 |
Hungary |
Spain, Italy, Korea, Latvia |
248 |
Latvia |
Spain, Hungary, Italy, Korea |
247 |
Spain |
Hungary, Italy, Korea, Latvia |
245 |
Italy |
Spain, Hungary, Israel, Latvia |
244 |
Israel |
Italy |
238 |
Lithuania |
Poland*, Portugal |
236 |
Poland* |
Lithuania, Portugal |
235 |
Portugal |
Lithuania, Poland* |
218 |
Chile |
|
Note: Adults aged 16-65; includes adults who were only administered the doorstep interview due to a language barrier (see Box 1.1 in Chapter 1). No adjustment is made to significance levels for multiple hypotheses testing. *Caution is required in interpreting results due to the high share of respondents with unusual response patterns. See the Note for Poland in the Reader’s Guide.
Countries and economies are ranked in descending order of their average proficiency in literacy.
Statistically significantly above the OECD average
Not statistically significantly different from the OECD average
Statistically significantly below the OECD average
Source: Table A.2.1 (L) in Annex A.
Countries performing well in one domain typically did well in other domains as well. The same five countries are at the top of the ranking in all domains: Finland, Japan, the Netherlands, Norway and Sweden. Seven other countries and economies scored above the OECD average in all domains: Canada, Denmark, England (United Kingdom), Estonia, the Flemish Region (Belgium), Germany and Switzerland. Eleven countries scored, on average, significantly below the OECD average across all domains: Chile, Croatia, France, Hungary, Israel, Italy, Korea, Lithuania, Poland, Portugal and Spain. Average proficiency in Chile was significantly lower than in all other participating countries and economies in all three domains.
Table 2.2. Comparison of countries and economies based on average proficiency in numeracy
Copy link to Table 2.2. Comparison of countries and economies based on average proficiency in numeracy
Mean score |
Comparison country/economy |
Countries and economies whose mean score is not statistically significantly different from the comparison country's/economy's score |
294 |
Finland |
|
291 |
Japan |
|
285 |
Sweden |
Netherlands, Norway |
285 |
Norway |
Netherlands, Sweden |
284 |
Netherlands |
Norway, Sweden |
281 |
Estonia |
Flemish Region (BE), Denmark |
279 |
Flemish Region (BE) |
Denmark, Estonia |
279 |
Denmark |
Flemish Region (BE), Estonia |
276 |
Switzerland |
Singapore |
274 |
Singapore |
Switzerland, Germany |
273 |
Germany |
Canada, Singapore |
271 |
Canada |
Germany, England (UK) |
268 |
England (UK) |
Austria, Canada, Czechia |
267 |
Czechia |
Austria, England (UK) |
267 |
Austria |
Czechia, England (UK) |
263 |
OECD average |
Latvia, Slovak Republic |
263 |
Latvia |
Slovak Republic |
261 |
Slovak Republic |
Ireland, Latvia |
260 |
Ireland |
New Zealand, Slovak Republic |
257 |
France |
Croatia, Hungary, New Zealand |
256 |
New Zealand |
France, Croatia, Hungary, Ireland, Korea |
254 |
Hungary |
France, Croatia, Korea, New Zealand |
254 |
Croatia |
France, Hungary, Korea, New Zealand |
253 |
Korea |
Croatia, Hungary, New Zealand |
250 |
Spain |
United States |
249 |
United States |
Spain, Israel, Italy, Lithuania |
246 |
Israel |
Italy, Lithuania, United States |
246 |
Lithuania |
Israel, Italy, United States |
244 |
Italy |
Israel, Lithuania, United States |
239 |
Poland* |
Portugal |
238 |
Portugal |
Poland* |
214 |
Chile |
|
Note: Adults aged 16-65; includes adults who were only administered the doorstep interview due to a language barrier (see Box 1.1 in Chapter 1). No adjustment is made to significance levels for multiple hypotheses testing. *Caution is required in interpreting results due to the high share of respondents with unusual response patterns. See the Note for Poland in the Reader’s Guide.
Countries and economies are ranked in descending order of their average proficiency in numeracy.
Statistically significantly above the OECD average
Not statistically significantly different from the OECD average
Statistically significantly below the OECD average
Source: Table A.2.1 (N) in Annex A.
Table 2.3. Comparison of countries and economies based on average proficiency in adaptive problem solving
Copy link to Table 2.3. Comparison of countries and economies based on average proficiency in adaptive problem solving
Mean score |
Comparison country/economy |
Countries and economies whose mean score is not statistically significantly different from the comparison country's/economy's score |
276 |
Finland |
Japan |
276 |
Japan |
Finland |
273 |
Sweden |
Norway |
271 |
Norway |
Sweden |
265 |
Netherlands |
Denmark, Estonia |
264 |
Denmark |
Estonia, Netherlands |
263 |
Estonia |
Flemish Region (BE), Denmark, Netherlands |
262 |
Flemish Region (BE) |
Germany, Estonia |
261 |
Germany |
Flemish Region (BE), Canada, England (UK) |
259 |
Canada |
Germany, England (UK) |
259 |
England (UK) |
Canada, Switzerland, Germany |
257 |
Switzerland |
England (UK) |
253 |
Austria |
New Zealand, Singapore |
252 |
Singapore |
Austria, Czechia, New Zealand |
251 |
OECD average |
Czechia, New Zealand, Singapore |
250 |
Czechia |
Ireland, New Zealand, Singapore, United States |
249 |
New Zealand |
Austria, Czechia, France, Ireland, Singapore, Slovak Republic, United States |
249 |
Ireland |
Czechia, France, New Zealand, Slovak Republic, United States |
248 |
France |
Ireland, New Zealand, Slovak Republic, United States |
247 |
United States |
Czechia, France, Ireland, New Zealand, Slovak Republic |
247 |
Slovak Republic |
France, Ireland, Latvia, New Zealand, United States |
244 |
Latvia |
Slovak Republic |
241 |
Spain |
Hungary |
241 |
Hungary |
Spain |
238 |
Korea |
Croatia, Israel |
236 |
Israel |
Croatia, Korea, Portugal |
235 |
Croatia |
Israel, Korea, Portugal |
233 |
Portugal |
Croatia, Israel, Italy, Lithuania |
231 |
Italy |
Lithuania, Portugal |
230 |
Lithuania |
Italy, Portugal |
226 |
Poland* |
|
218 |
Chile |
|
Note: Adults aged 16-65; includes adults who were only administered the doorstep interview due to a language barrier (see Box 1.1 in Chapter 1). No adjustment is made to significance levels for multiple hypotheses testing. *Caution is required in interpreting results due to the high share of respondents with unusual response patterns. See the Note for Poland in the Reader’s Guide.
Countries and economies are ranked in descending order of their average proficiency in adaptive problem solving.
Statistically significantly above the OECD average
Not statistically significantly different from the OECD average
Statistically significantly below the OECD average
Source: Table A.2.1 (A) in Annex A.
A similar picture emerges when looking at the association between average proficiency in different skill domains across countries. Figure 2.1 shows the relationship between average proficiency in literacy and numeracy. Average performance in the two domains is highly correlated, with a correlation coefficient of 0.881. The dashed line in Figure 2.1 represents the best estimate of numeracy proficiency for a given value of literacy proficiency, based on the underlying data from all participating countries and economies. For countries above the dashed line, average performance in numeracy is higher than what could be predicted based on average performance in literacy. One way of interpreting such results is saying that adults in these countries perform relatively better in numeracy than in literacy. The opposite can be said for countries that fall below the dashed line. A similar positive correlation between literacy and numeracy was also found in the first cycle of the Survey of Adult Skills (OECD, 2016[4]).2
Figure 2.1. Comparison of countries’ and economies’ average proficiency in literacy and numeracy
Copy link to Figure 2.1. Comparison of countries’ and economies’ average proficiency in literacy and numeracyNote: The correlation in this figure is based on countries’ and economies’ average proficiency in literacy and numeracy. *Caution is required in interpreting results due to the high share of respondents with unusual response patterns. See the Note for Poland in the Reader’s Guide.
Source: Table A.2.1. (L, N) in Annex A.
How adults are distributed across proficiency levels
To facilitate the interpretation of results, the 2023 Survey of Adult Skills divides the scales for the three domains, which range from 0 to 500, into proficiency levels. The scales for literacy and numeracy are divided into six levels: Below Level 1 and Levels 1 to 5, while adaptive problem solving does not include Level 5. For more information, readers are advised to consult the Reader’s Companion (OECD, 2024[5]).
The difficulty of the different assessment tasks is expressed on the same scale used for measuring the proficiency of respondents (see Chapter 1). This makes it possible to describe what adults at each proficiency level are able to do, based on the characteristics of the assessment tasks whose difficulty level matches that of respondents. These descriptions are provided in Table 2.4 for literacy, Table 2.5 for numeracy and Table 2.6 for adaptive problem solving.
As skills are valuable for individuals but also for societies, one priority for policy makers is the identification of adults who perform at low proficiency levels. This report defines low-performing adults as those who score at the two lowest levels (at or below Level 1) in all skill domains. Box 2.1 provides information on how the Survey of Adult Skills measures what adults with very low proficiency can do.
Table 2.4. Description of what adults can do at each proficiency level in literacy
Copy link to Table 2.4. Description of what adults can do at each proficiency level in literacy
Level |
Score range |
Percentage of adults scoring at each level (OECD average) |
What adults can do at this level |
---|---|---|---|
Level 5 |
Equal to or higher than 376 points |
1.1% |
At Level 5, the assessment provides no direct information on what adults can do. This is mostly because feasibility concerns (especially with respect to testing time) precluded the inclusion of highly difficult tasks involving complex interrelated goal structures, very long or complex document sets, or tools containing highly complex texts (e.g. extensive catalogues, complex menu structures, or lists of unstructured results from search engines), which require advanced skills to access and process the information they contain. These tasks, however, form part of the construct of literacy in today's world, and future assessments aiming at a better coverage of the upper-end of the proficiency scale may seek to include testing units tapping on literacy skills at Level 5. From the characteristics of the most difficult tasks at Level 4, some suggestions regarding what constitutes proficiency at Level 5 may be offered. Adults at Level 5 may be able to reason about the task itself, setting up reading goals based on complex and implicit requests. They can presumably search for and integrate information across multiple, dense texts containing distracting information in prominent positions. They are able to construct syntheses of similar and contrasting ideas or points of view; or evaluate evidence-based arguments and the reliability of unfamiliar information sources. Tasks at Level 5 may also require the application and evaluation of abstract ideas and relationships. Evaluating reliability of evidentiary sources and selecting not just topically relevant but also trustworthy information may be key to achievement. |
Level 4 |
326 to less than 376 points |
10.6% |
At Level 4, adults can read long and dense texts presented on multiple pages in order to complete tasks that involve access, understanding, evaluation and reflection about the text(s) contents and sources across multiple processing cycles. Adults at this level can infer what the task is asking based on complex or implicit statements. Successful task completion often requires the production of knowledge-based inferences. Texts and tasks at Level 4 may deal with abstract and unfamiliar situations. They often feature both lengthy contents and a large amount of distracting information, which is sometimes as prominent as the information required to complete the task. At this level, adults are able to reason based on intrinsically complex questions that share only indirect matches with the text contents, and/or require taking into consideration several pieces of information dispersed throughout the materials. Tasks may require evaluating subtle evidence-claims or persuasive discourse relationships. Conditional information is frequently present in tasks at this level and must be taken into consideration by the respondent. Response modes may involve assessing or sorting complex assertions. |
Level 3 |
276 to less than 326 points |
30.9% |
Adults at Level 3 are able to construct meaning across larger chunks of text or perform multi-step operations in order to identify and formulate responses. They can identify, interpret or evaluate one or more pieces of information, often employing varying levels of inferencing. They can combine various processes (accessing, understanding and evaluating) if required by the task. Adults at this level can compare and evaluate multiple pieces of information from the text(s) based on their relevance or credibility. Texts at this level are often dense or lengthy, including continuous, noncontinuous, mixed. Information may be distributed across multiple pages, sometimes arising from multiple sources that provide discrepant information. Understanding rhetorical structures and text signals becomes more central to successfully completing tasks, especially when dealing with complex digital texts that require navigation. The texts may include specific, possibly unfamiliar vocabulary and argumentative structures. Competing information is often present and sometimes salient, though no more than the target information. Tasks require the respondent to identify, interpret, or evaluate one or more pieces of information, and often require varying levels of inferencing. Tasks at Level 3 also often demand that the respondent disregard irrelevant or inappropriate text content to answer accurately. The most complex tasks at this level include lengthy or complex questions requiring the identification of multiple criteria, without clear guidance regarding what has to be done. |
Level 2 |
226 to less than 276 points |
31.4% |
At Level 2, adults are able to access and understand information in longer texts with some distracting information. They can navigate within simple multi-page digital texts to access and identify target information from various parts of the text. They can understand by paraphrasing or making inferences, based on single or adjacent pieces of information. Adults at Level 2 can consider more than one criterion or constraint in selecting or generating a response. The texts at this level can include multiple paragraphs distributed over one long or a few short pages, including simple websites. Noncontinuous texts may feature a two-dimension table or a simple flow diagram. Access to target information may require the use of signaling or navigation devices typical of longer print or digital texts. The texts may include some distracting information. Tasks and texts at this level sometimes deal with specific, possibly unfamiliar situations. Tasks require respondents to perform indirect matches between the text and content information, sometimes based on lengthy instructions. Some tasks statements provide little guidance regarding how to perform the task. Task achievement often requires the test taker to either reason about one piece of information or to gather information across multiple processing cycles. |
Level 1 |
176 to less than 226 points |
17.1% |
Adults at Level 1 are able to locate information on a text page, find a relevant link from a website, and identify relevant text among multiple options when the relevant information is explicitly cued. They can understand the meaning of short texts, as well as the organization of lists or multiple sections within a single page. The texts at level 1 may be continuous, noncontinuous, or mixed and pertain to printed or digital environments. They typically include a single page with up to a few hundred words and little or no distracting information. Noncontinuous texts may have a list structure (such as a web search engine results page) or include a small number of independent sections, possibly with pictorial illustrations or simple diagrams. Tasks at Level 1 involve simple questions providing some guidance as to what needs to be done and a single processing step. There is a direct, fairly obvious match between the question and target information in the text, although some tasks may require the examination of more than one piece of information. |
Below Level 1 |
Below 176 points |
8.9% |
Adults at Below Level 1 are able to process meaning at the sentence level. Given a series of sentences that increase in complexity, they can tell if a sentence does or does not make sense either in terms of plausibility in the real world (i.e. sentences describing events that can vs. cannot happen), or in terms of the internal logic of the sentence (i.e. sentences that are meaningful vs. not). Most adults at this level are also able to read short, simple paragraphs and, at certain points in text, tell which word among two makes the sentence meaningful and consistent with the rest of the passage. Finally, they can access single words or numbers in very short texts in order to answer simple and explicit questions. The texts at Below Level 1 are very short and include no or just a few familiar structuring devices such as titles or paragraph headers. They do not include any distracting information nor navigation devices specific to digital texts (e.g. menus, links or tabs). Tasks Below Level 1 are simple and very explicit regarding what to do and how to do it. These tasks only require understanding at the sentence level or across two simple adjacent sentences. When the text involves more than one sentence, the task merely requires dealing with target information in the form of a single word or phrase. |
Table 2.5. Description of what adults can do at each proficiency level in numeracy
Copy link to Table 2.5. Description of what adults can do at each proficiency level in numeracy
Level |
Score range |
Percentage of adults scoring at each level (OECD average) |
What adults can do at this level |
---|---|---|---|
Level 5 |
Equal to or higher than 376 points |
1.7% |
Adults at Level 5 can use and apply problem-solving strategies to analyse, evaluate, reason and critically reflect on complex and formal mathematical information, including dynamic representations. They demonstrate an understanding of statistical concepts and can critically reflect on whether a data set can be used to support or refute a claim. Adults at this level can determine the most appropriate graphical representation for relational data sets. |
Level 4 |
326 to less than 376 points |
12.2% |
Adults at Level 4 can use and apply a range of problem-solving strategies to access, analyse, reason, and critically reflect on and evaluate a broad range of mathematical information that is often presented in unfamiliar contexts. Such information may not be presented in an explicit manner. Adults at this level can devise and implement strategies to solve multi-step problems. This may involve reasoning about how to integrate concepts from different mathematical content areas or applying more complex and formal mathematical procedures. Adults at this level can:
|
Level 3 |
276 to less than 326 points |
30.6% |
Adults at Level 3 can access, act on, use, reflect on and evaluate authentic mathematical contexts. This requires making judgements about how to use the given information when developing a solution to a problem. The mathematical information may be less explicit, embedded in contexts that are not always commonplace, and use representations and terminology that are more formal and involve greater complexity. Adults at this level can complete tasks where mathematical processes require the application of two or more steps and where multiple conditions need to be satisfied. Tasks may also require the use, integration or manipulation of multiple data sources in order to undertake the mathematical analyses necessary for the specific task. Adults at this level can:
|
Level 2 |
226 to less than 276 points |
30.6% |
Adults at Level 2 can access, act on and use mathematical information, and evaluate simple claims, in tasks set in a variety of authentic contexts. They are able to interpret and use information presented in slightly more complex forms (e.g. doughnut charts, stacked bar graphs or linear scales) that includes more formal terminology and more distracting information. Adults at this level can carry out multi-step mathematical processes. Adults at this level can:
|
Level 1 |
176 to less than 226 points |
16.3% |
Adults at Level 1 demonstrate number sense involving whole numbers, decimals, and common fractions and percentages. They can access, act on and use mathematical information located in slightly more complex representations set in authentic contexts where the mathematical content is explicit and uses informal mathematical terminology with little text and minimal distracting information. They can devise simple strategies using one or two steps to determine the solution. Adults at this level can:
|
Below Level 1 |
Below 176 points |
8.6% |
Adults performing Below Level 1 demonstrate elementary whole-number sense and can access and use mathematical knowledge to solve single-step problems, where the information is presented using images or simple structured information set in authentic, commonplace contexts with little or no text or distracting information. The mathematical content is non-formal and explicit. Adults at this level can:
|
Table 2.6. Description of what adults can do at each proficiency level in adaptive problem solving
Copy link to Table 2.6. Description of what adults can do at each proficiency level in adaptive problem solving
Level |
Score range |
Percentage of adults scoring at each level (OECD average) |
What adults can do at this level |
---|---|---|---|
Level 4 |
Equal to or higher than 326 points |
5.0% |
Adults at this level are able to define the nature of problems in ill-structured and information-rich contexts. They integrate multiple sources of information and their interactions, identify and disregard irrelevant information, and formulate relevant cues. Adults identify and apply multi-step solutions towards one or more complex goals. They adapt the problem-solving process to changes even if these changes are not obvious, occur unexpectedly, or require a major reevaluation of the problem. Adults are able to distinguish between relevant and irrelevant changes, predict future developments of the problem situation, and consider multiple criteria simultaneously to judge whether the solution process is likely to lead to success. Adults at Level 4 engage in the following cognitive processes:
Adults at this level engage in the following metacognitive processes:
|
Level 3 |
276 to less than 326 points |
27.3% |
Adults at this level understand problems that are either more complex static problems or problems that have an average to high level of dynamics. They can solve problems with multiple constraints or problems that require the attainment of several goals in parallel. In problems that change and require adaptivity, adults deal with frequent and, to some extent, continuous changes. They discriminate between changes that are relevant and those that are less relevant or unrelated to the problem. Adults at this level can identify and apply multi-step solutions that integrate several important variables simultaneously and consider the impact of several problem elements on each other. In dynamically changing problems, they predict future developments in the problem situation based on information collected from past developments. They adapt their behaviour according to the predicted change. Adults at Level 3 engage in the following cognitive processes:
Adults at this level engage in the following metacognitive processes:
|
Level 2 |
226 to less than 276 points |
38.5% |
Adults at this level can identify and apply solutions that consist of several steps in problems that require considering one target variable to judge whether the problem has been solved. In dynamic problems that exhibit change, adults at this level can identify relevant information if they are prompted to specific aspects of the change or if changes are transparent, occur only one at a time, relate to a single problem feature, and are easily accessible. Problems at this level are presented in well-structured environments and contain only a few information elements with direct relevance to the problem. Minor impasses may be introduced but these can be resolved easily by adjusting the initial problem-solving procedure. Adults at Level 2 engage in the following cognitive processes:
Adults at this level engage in the following metacognitive processes:
|
Level 1 |
176 to less than 226 points |
21.5% |
Adults at this level are able to understand simple problems and develop and implement solutions to solve them. Problems contain a limited number of elements and little to no irrelevant information. Solutions at this level are simple and consist of a limited number of steps. Problems are embedded in a context that includes one or two sources of information and presents a single, explicitly defined goal. Adults at Level 1 engage in the following cognitive processes:
|
Below Level 1 |
Below 176 points |
7.7% |
Adults at this level understand very simple static problems situated within a clearly structured environment. Problems contain no invisible elements, no irrelevant information that might distract from the core of the problem, and typically only require a single-step solution. Adults at this proficiency level are able to engage in basic cognitive processes required to solve problems if explicit support is given and if they are prompted to do so. |
Box 2.1. Using components to assess what adults at low literacy and numeracy proficiency levels are able to do
Copy link to Box 2.1. Using components to assess what adults at low literacy and numeracy proficiency levels are able to doThe Survey of Adult Skills is designed to accurately measure the literacy and numeracy proficiency of all adults, including those who are low performing. In all assessment domains, tasks of varying levels of difficulty were designed to cover as much of the entire ability distribution as possible. To improve the precision of skills measurement at the lower end of the proficiency distribution, the 2023 assessments of literacy and numeracy included component tasks targeted at adults with very low levels of proficiency.
Reading components represent the basic set of decoding skills that are essential for extracting meaning from written texts. They comprise sentence comprehension (respondents are required to identify whether sentences make sense or not) and passage comprehension (respondents are required to read a short passage and identify words that give meaning to the sentence). Numeracy components assess number sense with two types of tasks (identifying quantities and recognising the bigger number in a set).
Performance in component tasks is integrated into the literacy and numeracy proficiency scales, which allows the lower end of the proficiency scale to be estimated with greater precision. At the same time, the results from these components can be analysed separately, to help understand and describe the skills and knowledge of adults with low levels of numeracy and literacy.
Source: OECD (2024[5]), Survey of Adult Skills: 2023 Reader’s Companion, https://doi.org/10.1787/3639d1e2-en; OECD (2021[6]), The Assessment Frameworks for Cycle 2 of the Programme for the International Assessments of Adult Competencies, https://doi.org/10.1787/4bc2342d-en.
On average, across participating OECD countries and economies, 26% of adults are low performing in literacy, meaning they scored at or below Level 1: 9% scored below Level 1 and 17% scored at Level 1 (Figure 2.2). Chile has the largest share of such low-performing adults (53%), while Japan has the smallest (10%). Further up the scale, 31% of adults scored at Level 2 and 31% at Level 3, on average across OECD countries and economies. At the highest proficiency levels, 11% of adults scored at Level 4 and 1% at Level 5. Finland has the largest share of adults at Levels 4 and 5 (35%), while Chile and Lithuania have the smallest (2%).
In numeracy, 25% of adults in participating OECD countries and economies are low performing on average. In particular, 9% of adults scored below Level 1 and 16% scored at Level 1 (Figure 2.3). The share of low-performing adults is largest in Chile (56%) and smallest in Japan (10%). On average, 31% of adults scored at Level 2, 31% at Level 3, 12% at Level 4 and 2% at Level 5. Finland is the country with the largest share of adults performing at Levels 4 and 5 (31%), while Chile has the smallest (2%).
In adaptive problem solving, on average, 29% of adults in participating OECD countries and economies are low performing (scoring at Level 1 or below). In particular, 8% of adults scored below Level 1 and 22% at Level 1 (Figure 2.4). Chile has the largest share of low-performing adults, at 56%, while Japan has the smallest, at 11%. In this domain, 38% of adults scored at Level 2 on average, 27% at Level 3 and 5% scored at Level 4. Finland has the largest share of adults scoring at Level 4 (13%) and Chile, Italy, Korea, Lithuania, Poland and the Slovak Republic have the smallest, below 1%.
Figure 2.2. Literacy proficiency among adults
Copy link to Figure 2.2. Literacy proficiency among adultsShare of 16-65 year-olds scoring at each proficiency level in literacy
Note: Includes adults who were only administered the doorstep interview due to a language barrier (see Box 1.1 in Chapter 1). *Caution is required in interpreting results due to the high share of respondents with unusual response patterns. See the Note for Poland in the Reader’s Guide.
Countries and economies are ranked in ascending order of the share of adults scoring at or below Level 1.
Source: Table A.2.2 (L) in Annex A.
Figure 2.3. Numeracy proficiency among adults
Copy link to Figure 2.3. Numeracy proficiency among adultsShare of 16-65 year-olds scoring at each proficiency level in numeracy
Note: Includes adults who were only administered the doorstep interview due to a language barrier (see Box 1.1 in Chapter 1). *Caution is required in interpreting results due to the high share of respondents with unusual response patterns. See the Note for Poland in the Reader’s Guide.
Countries and economies are ranked in ascending order of the share of adults scoring at or below Level 1.
Source: Table A.2.2 (N) in Annex A.
Figure 2.4. Proficiency in adaptive problem solving among adults
Copy link to Figure 2.4. Proficiency in adaptive problem solving among adultsShare of 16-65 year-olds scoring at each proficiency level in adaptive problem solving
Note: Includes adults who were only administered the doorstep interview due to a language barrier (see Box 1.1 in Chapter 1). *Caution is required in interpreting results due to the high share of respondents with unusual response patterns. See the Note for Poland in the Reader’s Guide.
Countries and economies are ranked in ascending order of the share of adults scoring at or below Level 1.
Source: Table A.2.2 (A) in Annex A.
As proficiency in literacy and numeracy is correlated (OECD, 2016[4]), adults with low proficiency in one domain are also likely to have low proficiency in other domains. Figure 2.5 shows the share of adults with low proficiency in all three domains. This share is highest in Chile (44%) and lowest in Japan (7%). Figure 2.5 also presents the percentage of low-performing adults in literacy and numeracy only. This share ranges between 48% in Chile and 8% in Japan.
Figure 2.5. Share of adults who are low performing in more than one domain
Copy link to Figure 2.5. Share of adults who are low performing in more than one domain16-65 year-olds scoring at or below Level 1 in more than one domain
Note: Includes adults who were only administered the doorstep interview due to a language barrier (see Box 1.1 in Chapter 1). *Caution is required in interpreting results due to the high share of respondents with unusual response patterns. See the Note for Poland in the Reader’s Guide.
Countries and economies are ranked in descending order of the share of adults performing at or below Level 1 in all three domains.
Source: Table A.2.3 in Annex A.
Aggregate inequality in the distribution of skills
Aggregate skills inequalities can be measured by the dispersion of the distribution of skills within each country or economy. One measure of such dispersion is the inter-decile range, i.e. the distance between the 90th percentile of the national skills distribution (the score below which 90% of adults perform) and the 10th percentile of the national skills distribution (the score below which 10% of adults perform).
On average, the inter-decile range in literacy in participating OECD countries and economies is 140 points (Figure 2.6, Panel A). Inequality is particularly pronounced in Singapore and the United States, where the inter-decile range exceeds 160 points. The distribution is more compressed in Lithuania and the Slovak Republic, where the inter-decile range is below 115 points.
In numeracy, the inter-decile range in participating OECD countries and economies averages 144 points (Figure 2.6, Panel B). The difference is widest in New Zealand, Portugal, Singapore and the United States, where the inter-decile range exceeds 160 points, while it is more compressed in Japan, Lithuania and the Slovak Republic, where the inter-decile range is below 130 points.
In adaptive problem solving, the inter-decile range averages 119 points (Figure 2.6, Panel C). Variation is widest in New Zealand and the United States, with the inter-decile range exceeding 140 points. The skills distribution is more compressed in Lithuania and the Slovak Republic, where the inter-decile range is lower than 100 points.
Figure 2.6. Inequality in the distribution of key information-processing skills
Copy link to Figure 2.6. Inequality in the distribution of key information-processing skillsDifference between the 90th and 10th percentile of the national skills distribution for literacy, numeracy and adaptive problem solving (90th percentile minus 10th percentile)
Note: Adults aged 16-65; includes adults who were only administered the doorstep interview due to a language barrier (see Box 1.1 in Chapter 1). *Caution is required in interpreting results due to the high share of respondents with unusual response patterns. See the Note for Poland in the Reader’s Guide.
Countries and economies are ranked in descending order of the difference between the 90th and the 10th percentile.
Source: Table A.2.1 (L, N, A) in Annex A.
The countries with the greatest inequality (widest inter-decile range) across all domains are New Zealand, Singapore and the United States; those with the lowest levels of inequality are Japan, Lithuania, the Slovak Republic and Sweden. Inequality in skills does not appear to be clearly correlated with average levels of proficiency, suggesting that education and training policies can succeed in equipping a large majority of the population with relatively high levels of skills. Countries where skills inequality is high also tend to be countries where parents’ level of education is more strongly related to skills proficiency of their offspring. This is consistent with findings from the previous cycle of the Survey of Adult Skills (Paccagnella, 2015[7]), and highlights the importance of helping people from disadvantaged backgrounds to achieve a more inclusive and fairer distribution of skills.
Socio-demographic differences in key information-processing skills
Copy link to Socio-demographic differences in key information-processing skillsMonitoring the skill endowment of different population subgroups can help countries and economies identify at-risk populations with low levels of foundation skills. Policy makers can use this information to identify, design and implement targeted policies to address the needs of low-skilled populations, as well as assessing the effectiveness of such measures. This section considers skill differences across groups defined in terms of age, educational attainment, gender, immigrant background and parental education.
Differences in skills proficiency related to age
Skills are not static. Over the course of a lifetime they can be acquired and developed, lose value, and even decline (Kautz et al., 2014[8]). Skills are influenced by the nurturing effects of the home, family, school and work environment (Kautz et al., 2014[8]); culture (Baltes, 1993[9]); genetic factors (Toga and Thompson, 2005[10]); effects related to ageing (Desjardins and Warnke, 2012[11]; Kautz et al., 2014[8]); and many other factors such as beliefs, attitudes and values. With increased life expectancy leading to longer working lives, it is more important than ever to understand how skills develop throughout people’s lives, including into old age.
The 2023 Survey of Adult Skills covers adults aged 16 to 65 (born between 1957 and 2007), spanning the end of compulsory schooling, through working age and up to the onset of retirement. Not all differences in skills among adults of different ages can be attributed to ageing itself (i.e. the consequences of growing older, including factors such as neurological development or behavioural maturation). Some of those differences can be due to cohort effects (which reflect the different experiences that adults born at different times go through, for example different educational policies) as well as to period effects (which capture influences that vary through time, like macro-economic conditions, or events like the COVID-19 pandemic). Cross-sectional data such as those collected in the Survey of Adult Skills, which only provide a snapshot of the skills of the population at a particular point in time, do not allow age, cohort and period effects to be disentangled. Chapter 3 uses information from the two cycles of the survey to separately investigate age and cohort effects.
Figure 2.7 shows the average proficiency of adults in different age groups in literacy, numeracy and adaptive problem solving. In the vast majority of countries and economies, adults aged 55-65 display the lowest average proficiency in all domains. The exceptions are New Zealand and Sweden, where 16-24 year-olds achieved the lowest average proficiency in numeracy in both countries. In literacy, New Zealand is the only country where 16-24 year-olds achieved the lowest average proficiency.
In most countries and economies, the highest average proficiency is observed among either 25-34 year-olds or 16-24 year-olds. Adults aged 25-34 achieved the highest average proficiency for literacy in 15 out of 31 countries and economies, in 14 for numeracy, and in 16 for adaptive problem solving while those aged 16-24 achieved the highest average proficiency in 14 countries and economies for literacy, 12 for numeracy, and 14 for adaptive problem solving. The exceptions in literacy are New Zealand, where 45-54 year-olds scored the highest on average, and the Slovak Republic, where the best-performing adults were 35-44 year-olds. In numeracy, 35-44 year-olds scored highest in Finland, Hungary and Sweden, while 45-54 year-olds scored highest in New Zealand and the Slovak Republic. In adaptive problem solving, 35-44 year-olds scored highest in the Slovak Republic.
Figure 2.7. Average proficiency in key information-processing skills, by age
Copy link to Figure 2.7. Average proficiency in key information-processing skills, by ageLiteracy, numeracy and adaptive problem solving
Note: Adults aged 16-65; includes adults who were only administered the doorstep interview due to a language barrier (see Box 1.1 in Chapter 1). *Caution is required in interpreting results due to the high share of respondents with unusual response patterns. See the Note for Poland in the Reader’s Guide.
Countries and economies are ranked in descending order of the average proficiency among 25-34 year-olds.
Source: Table A.2.4 (L, N, A) in Annex A.
Differences in skills proficiency related to educational attainment
Skills are developed, enhanced and accumulated over a lifetime through education and training opportunities. The various skills that adults possess at any given time are determined by their past access to learning opportunities. This process begins during early childhood education and care, continues through formal school education, and extends into adulthood through formal, non-formal and informal learning at work, in leisure time activities and at home. Assessing the relationship between education, learning opportunities and the skills proficiency of adults is therefore complex, as skill development is a dynamic process in which the skills acquired at earlier stages in life determine the learning at later stages in life (Cunha et al., 2006[12]).
Educational reforms and the expansion of access to formal education over the past decades have affected adults’ skills. Educational attainment levels have risen, a trend that is reflected in the expansion of tertiary attainment rates. Between 2000 and 2021, the share of 25-34 year-olds with tertiary attainment increased from 27% to 48% (OECD, 2022[13]). Opportunities for adults to advance their education, especially through online learning, have expanded. Although the COVID-19 pandemic had adverse effects, including school closures (Grewenig et al., 2021[14]; Werner and Woessmann, 2021[15]) and reduced informal learning opportunities (OECD, 2021[1]), it has also led to a substantial increase in online learning opportunities for adults (OECD, 2020[16]).
While skills can be developed through various education and training settings, formal education plays a pivotal role in the development of foundation skills (Desjardins, 2003[17]; OECD, 2013[18]). Historically, educational attainment or years of schooling have been used as the main proxy for the stock of human capital (Barro and Lee, 2013[19]; Hanushek and Woessmann, 2011[20]). However, this proxy is imperfect as it overlooks factors unrelated to educational attainment that affect skill development (Hanushek and Woessmann, 2011[20]; 2008[21]). Some of these factors are related to education systems – such as school quality, tracking, the share of privately funded schools and government systems (Green and Pensiero, 2016[22]; 2015[23]) – while others are related to the family, as elucidated further below.
Formal education has a crucial role in providing children, adolescents and adults with adequate proficiency in foundation skills like literacy, numeracy and problem solving. It is important to examine whether it is fulfilling this mission. This section analyses the relationship between the highest educational qualification obtained by adults and their foundation skills. However, it is important to acknowledge that the skills of the working-age population are not solely a product of their formal education and the design of school systems and selection processes (Silva et al., 2020[24]). They are also influenced by their occupation, their educational choices (in terms of the field of study, for instance), and the availability of non-formal and informal learning opportunities. It is also important to keep in mind that literacy, numeracy and adaptive problem solving are only some of the skills that are developed in formal education. In fact, formal education develops a wide range of skills, including subject-specific skills, social and emotional skills (OECD, 2024[25]), and creative thinking (OECD, 2024[26]).
Average proficiency, by educational attainment
The 2023 Survey of Adult Skills finds that higher levels of educational attainment are associated with higher average proficiency in literacy, numeracy and adaptive problem solving (Figure 2.8). Tertiary-educated adults scored 33 points higher in literacy, on average, than those with upper secondary attainment, who in turn scored, on average, 43 points higher than adults who did not complete upper secondary education (Figure 2.8, Panel A). These findings confirm the relationship already observed in the first cycle of the Survey of Adult Skills (OECD, 2016[4]). The analysis here is limited to those aged 25-65, as they are the most likely to have completed their formal education. Younger adults are a much more heterogeneous population, as only some of them have left education, while others will still be following a variety of educational paths.
Cross-country comparisons of the proficiency of adults at either the same level of educational attainment or at different levels, can be informative about how well different educational systems succeed in building their citizens’ skills. Adults with the same level of qualifications (e.g. tertiary) may perform better on average in one country or economy than their equally qualified peers in another. When adults with different levels of attainment (e.g. tertiary and upper secondary) are compared, those with upper secondary attainment in one country might be found to perform better than tertiary-educated adults in the other, on average.
Such comparisons show that the expected pattern of higher proficiency among individuals with higher levels of qualification does not always hold across borders: adults with relatively lower levels of educational attainment may have relatively high skills proficiency. For example, adults with upper secondary attainment in Finland scored 288 points in literacy on average, outperforming tertiary-educated adults in 19 out of 31 participating countries and economies (Figure 2.8, Panel A).
Conversely, highly educated adults in some countries might have lower proficiency than adults with lower attainment in other countries. For example, tertiary-educated adults in Chile scored an average of 249 points in literacy, below the average for those with upper secondary attainment in 17 other participating countries and economies.
These results underline the value of the Survey of Adult Skills in providing information about differences in skills between and within countries that statistics on educational attainment are unable to fully capture. The considerable differences can be attributed to cross-country differences in the quality of their education systems, but also to differences in the provision and organisation of education and lifelong learning opportunities. These include factors such as when learning takes place (e.g. childhood, youth or adulthood), where it takes place (formal, informal and non-formal), how learning is facilitated (e.g. whether there are barriers to participation), and what is learned (e.g. skills, attitudes and values) (OECD, 2021[1]).
Differences in proficiency between highly and low-educated adults
The size of the skills gap between highly educated adults (those with tertiary education) and low-educated ones (with below upper secondary education) varies across participating countries and economies (Figure 2.9). However, France, Germany, Singapore, Switzerland and the United States consistently have the widest gaps across all domains: exceeding 94 points in literacy, 102 points in numeracy and 76 points in adaptive problem solving. Conversely, the smallest gaps are in Croatia, Italy, Lithuania, Poland, the Slovak Republic and Spain for literacy, at 54 points and less, and in Croatia, Italy, Lithuania, the Slovak Republic and Spain for numeracy, at less than 61 points. In adaptive problem solving the five countries with the smallest average gaps (below 40 points) are Croatia, Estonia, Lithuania, the Slovak Republic and Spain.
Figure 2.9 also presents so-called “adjusted” differences. These take into account the fact that adults at different levels of education also differ in other dimensions that are independently related to skills proficiency. Box 2.2 describes the interpretation of adjusted versus unadjusted differences and the information they provide to countries, economies and policy makers. Estimates of adjusted differences control for age, gender, immigrant background, the language spoken at home and parents’ educational attainment. These socio-demographic characteristics, however, can only account for a small part of the observed gaps, confirming the important role played by education in explaining skills differences.
Figure 2.8. Average proficiency in key information-processing skills, by educational attainment
Copy link to Figure 2.8. Average proficiency in key information-processing skills, by educational attainmentLiteracy, numeracy and adaptive problem solving
Note: Adults aged 25-65; includes adults who were only administered the doorstep interview due to a language barrier (see Box 1.1 in Chapter 1). Respondents who were administered the doorstep interview reported the number of years they spent in the educational system and are included in the calculation of all estimates assuming a correspondence between years of education and educational levels as follows: No education or less than 10 years of education is considered equivalent to less than upper secondary education, between 11 and 13 years of education is considered equivalent to upper secondary education, and more than 13 years of education is considered equivalent to tertiary education. Educational attainment is based on the International Standard Classification of Education (ISCED) 2011, grouped into below upper secondary (ISCED 1, 2 and 3 short), upper secondary (ISCED 3 and 4) and tertiary (ISCED 5, 6, 7 and 8). Where possible, foreign qualifications are included as the closest corresponding level in the respective national education systems. *Caution is required in interpreting results due to the high share of respondents with unusual response patterns. See the Note for Poland in the Reader’s Guide.
Countries and economies are ranked in descending order of the proficiency of adults with below upper secondary attainment.
Source: Table A.2.5 (L, N, A) in Annex A.
Figure 2.9. Differences in key information-processing skills, by educational attainment
Copy link to Figure 2.9. Differences in key information-processing skills, by educational attainmentAdjusted and unadjusted differences in mean literacy, numeracy and adaptive problem solving scores between tertiary and below upper secondary educated adults (tertiary minus below upper secondary educated)
Note: Adults aged 25-65; includes adults who were only administered the doorstep interview due to a language barrier (see Box 1.1 in Chapter 1). Respondents who were administered the doorstep interview reported the number of years they spent in the educational system and are included in the calculation of all estimates assuming a correspondence between years of education and educational levels as follows: No education or less than 10 years of education is considered equivalent to less than upper secondary education, between 11 and 13 years of education is considered equivalent to upper secondary education, and more than 13 years of education is considered equivalent to tertiary education. Educational attainment is based on the International Standard Classification of Education (ISCED) 2011, grouped into below upper secondary (ISCED 1, 2 and 3 short), upper secondary (ISCED 3 and 4) and tertiary (ISCED 5, 6, 7 and 8). Where possible, foreign qualifications are included as the closest corresponding level in the respective national education systems. Unadjusted differences are the differences between the two averages for each contrast category. Adjusted differences are based on a regression model that takes into account differences associated with gender, age, immigrant background, language spoken at home and parents’ educational attainment. Doorstep interview cases for which parental education information is not collected are included in the regression by assigning them to a separate category. All differences are statistically significant at the 5% level. *Caution is required in interpreting results due to the high share of respondents with unusual response patterns. See the Note for Poland in the Reader’s Guide.
Countries and economies are ranked in descending order of the unadjusted difference in proficiency.
Source: Table A.2.5 (L, N, A) in Annex A.
Adults can achieve their highest formal qualification through several pathways. These pathways are, among other things, determined by the policies and practices used to select and sort students during formal schooling as well as by adults’ educational choices (OECD, 2020[27]). These choices include the orientation of an educational programme (general, vocational or combined) and the field of study. Box 2.3 discusses how differences in the field of study chosen by tertiary-educated adults are related to their proficiency in numeracy, focusing in particular on the skills of adults who studied science, technology, engineering or mathematics (STEM programmes).
Box 2.2. Adjusted versus non-adjusted differences: Understanding variations in skills proficiency across different socio-demographic groups
Copy link to Box 2.2. Adjusted versus non-adjusted differences: Understanding variations in skills proficiency across different socio-demographic groupsThis chapter presents differences in proficiency between subgroups both before (unadjusted) and after (adjusted), accounting for differences in socio-demographic characteristics that are associated with skills proficiency. Unadjusted differences inform policy makers about the size of the real gaps in performance between groups. Adjusted differences are informative about the factors that are driving and potentially explaining these observed gaps. Adjusted differences are estimated through linear regressions. The estimated coefficient of the variable of interest captures adjusted differences. Large differences between adjusted and unadjusted coefficients suggest that the groups analysed differ according to other factors that are correlated with skills proficiency.
For example, proficiency differences between low- and highly educated adults might partly be due to the fact that both groups differ according to other dimensions which are independently correlated with proficiency, for instance, average age. Data from the 2023 Survey of Adult Skills show that in some countries, adults aged 55-65 are over-represented among the low-educated. For example, in France, 33% of low-educated adults are over 55, while only 16% of highly educated adults fall in that age range (see Table B.3.17 in Annex B). To the extent that older adults, irrespective of their level of education, perform worse on the assessment, the average score of low-educated adults would be impacted by something that has little to do with education levels. Adjusted differences control for this by comparing the proficiency of low-educated and highly educated adults with similar characteristics. This gives a more accurate picture of the extent to which observed differences can be attributed to differences in levels of education.
In this chapter, adjusted estimates control for differences in age, educational attainment, gender, immigrant background, language spoken at home and parents’ educational attainment, as these factors are plausibly independently correlated with proficiency. Comparing adjusted and unadjusted differences highlights the extent to which the adults that are compared differ along the characteristics that are controlled for. For example, in Estonia, unadjusted and adjusted differences in literacy (61 and 59 points, Figure 2.9) are very similar, suggesting that low-educated and highly educated do not differ much in terms of socio-demographic characteristics, or that the effect of any differences in characteristics cancel each other out. In contrast, in Finland, the large difference between the unadjusted and adjusted differences in literacy (89 and 51 points, Figure 2.9) hints at the fact that low- and highly educated adults differ along other dimensions, beyond their education.
Box 2.3. Field of study choices and proficiency in numeracy among tertiary-educated adults
Copy link to Box 2.3. Field of study choices and proficiency in numeracy among tertiary-educated adultsIndividuals can choose between various fields in their tertiary education.1 Over the past decades, technological progress has increased the demand for adults with the right mix of skills to work in technology-rich environments. This has raised the economic returns of obtaining degrees in science, technology, engineering and mathematics (STEM) (OECD, 2022[13]).2 Several countries, including Denmark and the United States, have launched initiatives to increase the number of students enrolled in STEM programmes; encourage learners’ interest in such fields; and foster diversity, equity and inclusion in STEM fields (OECD, n.d.[28]; Office of Science and Technology Policy, 2021[29]).
On average across OECD countries and economies, 27% of tertiary-educated adults have a STEM degree. This share varies from more than 36% in Germany and Singapore to around 21% in the Netherlands (Figure 2.10).
Adults’ field of study is correlated with differences in numeracy proficiency. This can be attributed both to the fact that access and success to STEM programmes require a high level of numeracy skills, and to the fact that STEM programmes (and employment in STEM sectors) help develop such skills. Among tertiary-educated 25-65 year-olds, the average numeracy proficiency of those who studied in a STEM field is 304 points compared to 285 points for those who studied in non-STEM fields (Figure 2.11).
Significant differences in average numeracy proficiency between adults with a STEM and a non-STEM tertiary education range from 28 points in Japan to 5 points in Croatia (see Table A.2.6 (N) in Annex A). For other domains, adults who studied a STEM field have an average advantage of 6 points in literacy and 10 points in adaptive problem solving (see Table A.2.6 (L) and Table A.2.6 (A) in Annex A).
Figure 2.10. Share of tertiary-educated adults who studied STEM fields
Copy link to Figure 2.10. Share of tertiary-educated adults who studied STEM fieldsShare of tertiary-educated 25-65 year-olds who obtained their highest qualification in a science, technology, engineering or mathematics (STEM) field
Note: Does not include adults who were only administered the doorstep interview due to a language barrier, as information on field of study was not collected for those respondents (see Box 1.1 in Chapter 1). STEM stands for science, technology, engineering and mathematics. Educational attainment is based on the International Standard Classification of Education (ISCED) 2011, grouped into below upper secondary (ISCED 1, 2 and 3 short), upper secondary (ISCED 3 and 4) and tertiary (ISCED 5, 6, 7 and 8). Where possible, foreign qualifications are included as the closest corresponding level in the respective national education systems. *Caution is required in interpreting results due to the high share of respondents with unusual response patterns. See the Note for Poland in the Reader’s Guide.
Countries and economies are ranked in descending order based on the percentage of tertiary-educated adults who studied STEM fields.
Source: Table B.3.2 in Annex B.
Figure 2.11. Average numeracy proficiency among tertiary-educated adults, by field of study
Copy link to Figure 2.11. Average numeracy proficiency among tertiary-educated adults, by field of studyNote: Adults aged 25-65; does not include adults who were only administered the doorstep interview due to a language barrier, as information on field of study was not collected for those respondents (see Box 1.1 in Chapter 1). STEM stands for science, technology, engineering and mathematics. Educational attainment is based on the International Standard Classification of Education (ISCED) 2011, grouped into below upper secondary (ISCED 1, 2 and 3 short), upper secondary (ISCED 3 and 4) and tertiary (ISCED 5, 6, 7 and 8). Where possible, foreign qualifications are included as the closest corresponding level in the respective national education systems. Darker colours denote differences that are statistically significant at the 5% level. *Caution is required in interpreting results due to the high share of respondents with unusual response patterns. See the Note for Poland in the Reader’s Guide.
Countries and economies are ranked in descending order of the proficiency of tertiary-educated adults who studied STEM fields.
Source: Table A.2.6 (N) in Annex A.
1. Within the International Standard Classification of Education (ISCED), programmes and related qualifications can be classified by the field of education and training as well as the level. The 2023 Survey of Adult Skills distinguishes between the following fields of study: economics, business and administration; law; health; welfare; social and behavioural sciences; journalism and information; information and communication technologies (ICT); natural sciences, mathematics and statistics; engineering and manufacturing; construction; agriculture, forestry, fisheries and environmental studies; personal and community services; security and transport; education and teacher training; humanities, languages and arts; and no main area of study or emphasis (if it was a general education programme).
2. In the 2023 Survey of Adult Skills, adults educated in a STEM field are defined as having obtained their highest educational qualification in one of the following fields: ICT; natural sciences, mathematics and statistics; engineering and manufacturing; or construction.
Differences in skills proficiency related to gender
Promoting gender equality is not just a matter of social justice: it also has the potential to enhance growth, productivity, competitiveness and the sustainability of economies (Klasen, 2002[30]). While efforts have been made to reduce gender gaps in various aspects of life, disparities persist, including in academic performance (OECD, 2023[31]; 2016[4]), financial literacy (Monticone, 2023[32]), physical skills (Borgonovi, Seitz and Vogel, 2022[33]), labour-market outcomes (OECD, 2023[34]) and leadership positions (OECD, 2019[35]).
In recent years, historical gender gaps in education that originally favoured men have narrowed and, in some cases, even reversed. For example, the share of women aged 25-34 with tertiary education has consistently risen over the past decades, overtaking the share of men; as of 2023, the gap in OECD countries is 13 percentage points in favour of women, on average, and widening (Lee and Lee, 2016[36]; OECD, 2024[37]; 2021[38]). Despite this, women continue to face considerable disadvantages. For example, women and girls, on average, score lower in mathematics and numeracy performance (OECD, 2023[31]; 2019[39]). Women are also under-represented in STEM programmes, as men and women tend to make different choices about what to study (OECD, 2023[40]; 2022[13]). Box 2.4 shows differences in the field-of-study choices between men and women, along with associated gaps in numeracy proficiency. There are also worrying gender gaps to the disadvantage of men and boys. For example, among 15-year-olds, boys make up a larger share of low performers in reading (31% of boys and 22% of girls) (OECD, 2023[31]) and on average 57% of students who repeat a grade are boys (OECD, 2024[37]).
In the 2023 Survey of Adult Skills women scored higher in literacy, while men scored higher in numeracy and adaptive problem solving, on average. Men are also more likely to be low performers in literacy while women are more likely to be low performers in numeracy.
Box 2.4. Educational choices and gender differences
Copy link to Box 2.4. Educational choices and gender differencesMen and women make different educational choices. At the age of 15, girls are less likely than boys to plan to pursue a career that involves a lot of mathematics. Later in life, women are less likely to enrol in STEM programmes (OECD, 2023[40]; 2022[13]), and they are also more likely to drop out of them before graduating (OECD, 2022[13]).
This leads to the under-representation of women in the technology sector, which matters not only for individual outcomes, but also for societies more broadly (OECD, 2024[41]). For example, in the European Union, 18% of information and communications technology (ICT) specialists are women (OECD, 2024[41]). Under-representation of women leads to less diverse work environments which can have important consequences. For example, biases in artificial intelligence (AI) systems are attributed to biased data used to train algorithms, and de-biasing AI systems requires the awareness that these biases exist, which is more likely with diverse teams (Vallee, 2021[42]).
On average, across participating OECD countries, less than one-third of tertiary-educated adults who graduated from a STEM field are women (Figure 2.12). In contrast, women are over-represented in non-STEM fields (65%). The share of women with a STEM degree is highest in Portugal at 39% and lowest in Japan at 15%. Women outnumber men among the graduates of non-STEM fields in all countries, with shares ranging from 73% in Lithuania and Poland to 55% in Switzerland.
Figure 2.12. Share of women among STEM and non-STEM graduates
Copy link to Figure 2.12. Share of women among STEM and non-STEM graduatesShare of tertiary-educated 25-65 year-olds who graduated from each field of study who are women
Note: Does not include adults who were only administered the doorstep interview due to a language barrier, as information on field of study was not collected for those respondents (see Box 1.1 in Chapter 1). STEM stands for science, technology, engineering and mathematics. Educational attainment is based on the International Standard Classification of Education (ISCED) 2011, grouped into below upper secondary (ISCED 1, 2 and 3 short), upper secondary (ISCED 3 and 4) and tertiary (ISCED 5, 6, 7 and 8). Where possible, foreign qualifications are included as the closest corresponding level in the respective national education systems. *Caution is required in interpreting results due to the high share of respondents with unusual response patterns. See the Note for Poland in the Reader’s Guide.
Countries and economies are ranked in descending order of the percentage of STEM graduates who are women.
Source: Table B.3.4 in Annex B.
When comparing tertiary-educated men and women who have studied STEM fields, data from the 2023 Survey of Adult Skills show that, on average, women score 8 points lower in numeracy (Figure 2.13). This gap is narrower than the gap among all tertiary-educated adults (16 points; see Table A.2.9 (N) in Annex A). Accounting for other relevant characteristics only marginally helps to explain this gap.
Among tertiary-educated adults who have chosen STEM fields, women have a small and statistically insignificant advantage in literacy over men (1 point); this gap increases to 2 points and becomes statistically significant after accounting for differences in other background characteristics. In contrast, men in this group have a small but statistically significant advantage over women in adaptive problem solving (3 points), which reduces to 2 points after accounting for relevant socio-demographic characteristics (see Table A.2.9 (L, A) in Annex A).
These results show that the gender gap in numeracy persists even when comparing men and women who made similar educational choices. Factors other than the observable socio-demographic characteristics examined in this report would need to be investigated in order to understand the reasons behind such differences and to design initiatives that aim at reducing this gender gap. The first cycle of the Survey of Adult Skills also found an advantage for men in numeracy (10 points) among adults working in STEM occupations (OECD, 2015[43]).
Figure 2.13. Gender differences in numeracy among STEM graduates
Copy link to Figure 2.13. Gender differences in numeracy among STEM graduatesAdjusted and unadjusted differences in average numeracy scores between tertiary-educated men and women who studied STEM fields (men minus women)
Note: Adults aged 25-65. Does not include adults who were only administered the doorstep interview due to a language barrier, as information on field of study was not collected for those respondents (see Box 1.1 in Chapter 1). STEM stands for science, technology, engineering and mathematics. Educational attainment is based on the International Standard Classification of Education (ISCED) 2011, grouped into below upper secondary (ISCED 1, 2 and 3 short), upper secondary (ISCED 3 and 4) and tertiary (ISCED 5, 6, 7 and 8). Where possible, foreign qualifications are included as the closest corresponding level in the respective national education systems. Unadjusted differences are the differences between the two averages for each contrast category. Adjusted differences are based on a regression model that takes into account differences associated with age, immigrant background, language spoken at home and parents’ educational attainment. Darker colours denote differences that are statistically significant at the 5% level. *Caution is required in interpreting results due to the high share of respondents with unusual response patterns. See the Note for Poland in the Reader’s Guide.
Countries and economies are ranked in descending order of the unadjusted difference between men and women who studied STEM fields. Source: Table A.2.9 (N) in Annex A.
Differences in proficiency between men and women
On average across participating OECD countries and economies, women scored 3 points higher than men in literacy, while men outscored women by 10 points in numeracy and 2 points in adaptive problem solving (Figure 2.14). After accounting for relevant background characteristics, the adjusted gender gap shrinks to 1 point in favour of women in literacy (which is not statistically significant), but widens to 12 points in numeracy and 4 points in adaptive problem solving in favour of men. One potential explanation for this increase in the size of the adjusted coefficients in numeracy and adaptive problem solving is gender differences in educational attainment. For example, on average, 48% of women are tertiary educated, but only 40% of men are (see Table B.3.3 in Annex B). As higher levels of education are normally associated with higher proficiency, adjusted differences control for the fact that women appear to underperform in the assessment, compared to what one would expect based on their level of education.
In literacy, women in 13 countries (Croatia, Estonia, Finland, France, Germany, Hungary, Israel, Latvia, Lithuania, the Netherlands, New Zealand, Norway and Poland) scored significantly higher than men on average before accounting for relevant background characteristics, while in Singapore, men achieved significantly higher average literacy proficiency than women. In numeracy, men scored, on average, significantly higher than women in 27 countries and economies. In adaptive problem solving, in nine countries and economies (Austria, Chile, England [United Kingdom], the Flemish Region [Belgium], Korea, Lithuania, Portugal, Singapore and Switzerland) men scored significantly higher than women, on average. The direction of the gender gap in numeracy is the same as in the first cycle of the survey (OECD, 2019[39]); for literacy, however, the gender gap has reversed.
On average across participating OECD countries and economies, the direction of the gender gaps observed in the 2023 Survey of Adult Skills in literacy and numeracy mirrors those found in the OECD Programme for International Student Assessment (PISA).3 In 2022, 15-year-old boys scored on average 9 points above girls in the PISA mathematics assessment, while girls had a 24-point advantage in reading (OECD, 2023[31]). Although the average gender gaps in proficiency in PISA and the 2023 Survey of Adult Skills point in the same direction, the magnitude of the gaps cannot be directly compared, as PISA and the Survey of Adult Skills use two different reporting scales. However, it is possible to express gender gaps in the two surveys relative to their respective average standard deviations (55 points in literacy and 58 points in numeracy in the 2023 Survey of Adult Skills; and 90 points in mathematics and 101 points in reading in PISA). When dividing the gender gap by the standard deviation, the resulting effect size in reading is 24% of a standard deviation among 15-year-olds in PISA, compared to 5% of a standard deviation in literacy among adults in the 2023 Survey of Adult Skills. This suggests that the gender gap in literacy among adults is smaller than the gender gap in reading for 15-year old students. In contrast, the effect size is 10% of a standard deviation for mathematics in PISA, compared to 16% of a standard deviation for numeracy in the 2023 Survey of Adult Skills, suggesting a widening of the gap among adults. These findings are in line with previous studies that compared the proficiency of 15-year-olds in PISA with that of 27-year-olds in the first cycle of the Survey of Adult Skills (Borgonovi, Choi and Paccagnella, 2021[44]).
Observed gender disparities in academic performance tend to develop early in life due to prevailing stereotypical expectations and cultural norms within the social and cultural environment (González de San Román and De la Rica, 2020[45]). The empirical evidence suggests that gender stereotypes about mathematics ability emerge before any actual differences in achievement are observed (Cvencek, Meltzoff and Greenwald, 2011[46]). Boys also tend to be more confident than girls in tackling maths tasks (OECD, 2013[47]), while girls exhibit higher levels of mathematics anxiety (OECD, 2015[48]). Men are more likely than women to select fields of studies – and consequently occupations – that require more intense use of numeracy skills, which fosters their development. These could explain the widening gap in numeracy. The smaller literacy gap among adults may be explained by literacy being considered a more transversal skill that is developed irrespective of the field of study (Borgonovi, Choi and Paccagnella, 2021[44]).
Figure 2.14. Gender differences in key information-processing skills
Copy link to Figure 2.14. Gender differences in key information-processing skillsAdjusted and unadjusted differences in average literacy, numeracy and adaptive problem solving scores between men and women (men minus women)
Note: Adults aged 16-65. Includes adults who were only administered the doorstep interview due to a language barrier (see Box 1.1 in Chapter 1). Unadjusted differences are the differences between the two averages for each contrast category. Adjusted differences are based on a regression model that takes into account differences associated with education, age, immigrant background, language spoken at home and parents’ educational attainment. Doorstep interview cases for which parental education information is not collected are included in the regression by assigning them to a separate category. Darker colours denote differences that are statistically significant at the 5% level. *Caution is required in interpreting results due to the high share of respondents with unusual response patterns. See the Note for Poland in the Reader’s Guide. Countries and economies are ranked in descending order of the unadjusted proficiency difference between men and women.
Source: Table A.2.7 (L, N, A) in Annex A.
Figure 2.15. Share of low performers in key information-processing skills, by gender
Copy link to Figure 2.15. Share of low performers in key information-processing skills, by genderShare of adults who scored at or below Level 1 in literacy, numeracy and adaptive problem solving
Note: Adults aged 16-65. Includes adults who were only administered the doorstep interview due to a language barrier (see Box 1.1 in Chapter 1). Darker colours denote differences that are statistically significant at the 5% level. *Caution is required in interpreting results due to the high share of respondents with unusual response patterns. See the Note for Poland in the Reader’s Guide.
Countries and economies are ranked in descending order of the share of women scoring at or below Level 1.
Source: Table A.2.7 (L, N, A) in Annex A.
Gender differences among low performers
As higher proficiency in literacy, numeracy and adaptive problem solving are associated with a number of important economic and social outcomes for both individuals and countries (see Chapter 4), it is important to identify those scoring at low proficiency levels. This section examines differences in the percentage of low-performing men and women, i.e. those scoring at or below Level 1.
Figure 2.15 shows the percentage of low performers among men and women for all three skill domains. In literacy, 27% of men and 25% of women are low performing. In numeracy the shares are 24% of men and 26% of women. In adaptive problem solving, the share of low performers is not statistically different across genders (29% each).
Low literacy performance is more prevalent among men than among women in 16 countries. In Estonia, Hungary, Israel, Latvia and New Zealand, gender differences are particularly pronounced, with men 5 percentage points more likely than women to be low performers. The opposite is true in Singapore, where only 28% of men are low performing, compared to 31% of women.
In numeracy, the share of low-performing women is significantly higher than that of men in nine countries and economies (Austria, Canada, Chile, England [United Kingdom], France, Korea, Portugal, Singapore and Switzerland). In adaptive problem solving, Estonia has significantly more low-performing men, while Chile, Korea, Portugal and Singapore have significantly more low-performing women.
The over-representation of low performers among men in literacy and among women in numeracy mirrors findings from PISA. Among 15-year-olds, 31% of boys and 22% of girls are low performing in reading. In contrast, in mathematics, slightly more girls than boys are low performing (32% versus 31%).
Differences in skills proficiency related to immigrant background
Migration flows in OECD countries and economies have reached an unprecedented level, with over 6 million new permanent immigrants in 2022 (OECD, 2023[49]).4 By mid-2023, OECD countries had recorded an influx of around 5 million refugees fleeing the war of aggression of Russia against Ukraine.5 In light of these circumstances, public and policy discussions regarding the support and facilitation of migrants’ integration into education systems, labour markets and society remain prominent on the political agenda.
This section distinguishes between adults who are foreign-born of foreign-born parents, native-born of foreign-born parents and native-born of native-born parents. On average, native-born adults of native-born parents make up 75% of the adult population across countries and economies participating in the Survey of Adult Skills (see Table B.3.10 in Annex B).6 These shares range from 46% in Switzerland to 97% each in Japan and Korea and 99% in Poland. Foreign-born adults of foreign-born parents comprise the second largest group with 15%, with shares ranging between 1% in the Slovak Republic to 33% in Switzerland and New Zealand. Finally, native-born adults of foreign-born parents comprise 5% of respondents on average, with shares ranging from 1% in Czechia, Italy, Lithuania and Spain to 18% in Israel. When interpreting the average proficiency of immigrant groups or proficiency differences between native-born adults of native-born parents and immigrant groups, the size of the immigrant population should be considered (see Table B.3.10 in Annex B).
Average proficiency, by immigrant background
The 2023 Survey of Adult Skills found substantial differences in proficiency between adults with different immigrant backgrounds in each domain. In literacy, on average, native-born adults of native-born parents scored 267 points, native-born adults of foreign-born parents scored 260 points and foreign-born adults of foreign-born parents scored 222 points (Figure 2.16, Panel A). Average scores of native-born adults of native-born parents range from 219 points in Chile to 307 points in Finland. Among native-born adults of foreign-born parents, scores range from 233 points in Czechia to 289 points in Ireland. Among foreign-born adults of foreign-born parents, scores range from 188 points in Korea to 254 in Ireland.
In numeracy, on average, native-born adults of native-born parents scored 269 points, native-born adults of foreign-born parents scored 258 points and foreign-born adults of foreign-born parents scored 230 points (Figure 2.16, Panel B). Among native-born adults of native-born parents, scores range from 215 in Chile to 302 points in Finland. Among native-born adults of foreign-born parents, scores range from 224 points in Portugal to 280 points in Switzerland and 281 points in Canada. Among foreign-born adults of foreign-born parents, scores range between 190 points in Czechia and more than 260 points in Estonia (262), Canada (263) and Singapore (264).
In adaptive problem solving, on average, native-born adults of native-born parents scored 256 points, native-born adults of foreign-born parents scored 249 points and foreign-born adults of foreign-born parents scored 223 points (Figure 2.16, Panel C). Among native-born adults of native-born parents, scores range from 218 points in Chile to 280 points or higher in Norway (280 points), Sweden (282 points) and Finland (283 points). Among native-born adults of foreign-born parents, scores range from 224 points in Czechia to 271 points in both Ireland and Norway. Among foreign-born adults of foreign-born parents, scores range between 201 points in Czechia and 245 points or higher in Canada and Ireland (245 points each) and Sweden (246 points).
Figure 2.17 compares the literacy proficiency of native-born adults of native-born parents to that of foreign-born adults of foreign-born parents and native-born adults of foreign-born parents. On average, foreign-born adults of foreign-born parents scored 44 points lower than native-born adults of native-born parents (Panel A). The largest difference is observed in Finland (105 points), the lowest in Latvia (3 points) and Lithuania (1 point). Native-born adults of foreign-born parents scored 8 points lower than native-born adults of native-born parents in literacy, on average (Panel B). The largest difference was in the Flemish Region (Belgium), at 38 points. In contrast, in Ireland, native-born adults of foreign-born parents scored on average 25 points higher than native-born adults of native-born parents. Native-born adults of foreign-born parents also scored significantly higher than native-born adults of native-born parents in Canada (9 points) and Israel (6 points).
These differences (which are often substantial) partly stem from the fact that adults with a migration background are subject to multiple sources of disadvantage. It is therefore particularly important to take into account differences in other socio-demographic characteristics when interpreting skills differences among adults with different immigrant backgrounds. Accounting for these socio-demographic factors reduces the gap in literacy skills between foreign-born adults of foreign-born parents and native-born adults of native-born parents from 44 to 28 points (Figure 2.17, Panel A). In Finland, where the unadjusted proficiency gap between these groups is widest, it narrows from 105 to 61 points after accounting for socio-demographic characteristics. Among native-born adults of foreign-born parents, adjusted estimates reduce the gap from 8 to 4 points (Figure 2.17, Panel B).
Differences in numeracy and adaptive problem solving between native-born adults of native-born parents and adults with different immigrant backgrounds are provided in Table A.2.10 (N, A) in Annex A. In numeracy and adaptive problem solving the gap between native-born adults of native-born parents and foreign-born adults of foreign-born parents is 38 points and 32 points, while the gap between native-born adults of native-born parents to native-born adults of foreign-born parents is 10 points and 6 points.
The Survey of Adult Skills assesses adults’ proficiency in literacy, numeracy and adaptive problem solving in a particular language(s) spoken in the participating countries and economies. Proficiency in the language of the assessment is therefore a crucial factor that must be taken into account when interpreting the results. Individuals who lack the language proficiency to participate in the survey are nonetheless included in this analysis, as some minimal information has been collected on them through the doorstep interview (see Box 1.1. in Chapter 1). Unsurprisingly, the large majority of adults that were only able to answer the doorstep interview were foreign-born (see Table B.3.18 in Annex B and Chapters 5 and 6 in OECD (2024[5])). Immigrants who speak the language in which the test is administered can naturally be expected to display higher proficiency than those who do not.
Figure 2.16. Average proficiency in key information-processing skills, by immigrant background
Copy link to Figure 2.16. Average proficiency in key information-processing skills, by immigrant backgroundLiteracy, numeracy and adaptive problem solving
Note: Adults aged 16-65. Includes adults who were only administered the doorstep interview due to a language barrier (see Box 1.1 in Chapter 1). Foreign-born of foreign-born parents include adults who reported being foreign-born in the doorstep interview, while native-born doorstep respondents are not part of any of the groups presented in this figure. Results for some groups in Chile, Finland, Hungary, Japan, Korea, Poland and the Slovak Republic are not reported because there are too few observations to provide reliable estimates. *Caution is required in interpreting results due to the high share of respondents with unusual response patterns. See the Note for Poland in the Reader’s Guide. Countries and economies are ranked in descending order of the average proficiency of native-born adults of native-born parents.
Source: Table A.2.10 (L, N, A) in Annex A.
Figure 2.17. Differences in literacy proficiency, by immigrant background
Copy link to Figure 2.17. Differences in literacy proficiency, by immigrant backgroundAdjusted and unadjusted differences in average literacy between immigrant groups (native-born adults of native-born parents minus foreign-born adults of foreign-born parents, native-born adults of native-born parents minus native-born adults of foreign-born parents)
Note: Adults aged 16-65. Includes adults who were only administered the doorstep interview due to a language barrier (see Box 1.1 in Chapter 1). Foreign-born of foreign-born parents include adults who reported being foreign-born in the doorstep interview, while native-born doorstep respondents are not part of any of the groups presented in this figure. Unadjusted differences are the differences between the two averages for each contrast category. Adjusted differences are based on a regression model that takes into account differences associated with gender, education, age, language spoken at home and parents’ educational attainment. Doorstep interview cases for which parental education information is not collected are included in the regression by assigning them to a separate category. Darker colours denote differences that are statistically significant at the 5% level. Results for some groups in Chile, Finland, Hungary, Japan, Korea, Poland and the Slovak Republic are not reported because there are too few observations to provide reliable estimates. *Caution is required in interpreting results due to the high share of respondents with unusual response patterns. See the Note for Poland in the Reader’s Guide.
Countries and economies are ranked in descending order of the unadjusted difference between immigrant groups.
Source: Table A.2.10 (L) in Annex A.
Therefore, the reduction in these gaps before and after accounting for relevant background characteristics may be explained by migrants being less likely to speak the language of the assessment (or having lower proficiency in the language of the assessment) and lower levels of education on average. For example, among foreign-born adults of foreign-born parents, only 45% speak the language of the assessment at home (see Table B.3.11 in Annex B), and the share of those without upper secondary education is 9 percentage points higher than among adults who are native-born of native-born parents (see Table B.3.12 in Annex B). As discussed below, the survey assesses some of these characteristics and their association with migrants’ proficiency. However, there are multiple other factors that may further contribute to explaining proficiency gaps. Among these are the quality of school education in their country of birth; differences in school resources between schools with a high and low proportion of migrants in the host country; and differences in financial, human, and social and cultural capital between parents of migrants and non-migrants.
Individual migration characteristics and differences in proficiency
Migrant populations are highly heterogeneous across and within countries. Even within the same host country, migrants have different migration histories. The 2023 Survey of Adult Skills collects detailed information on the migration history of adults. Migrants can be characterised by the following factors: whether they speak the language of the host country, where they obtained their education, their age at arrival and the duration of their stay in the host country. These migration-related factors are relevant in explaining differences in performance in literacy, numeracy and adaptive problem solving.
As stated above, foreign-born adults of foreign-born parents constitute the largest group of those with an immigrant background. On average, 45% of foreign-born adults speak the language of the host country at home; 26% had arrived within the last five years, meaning 74% have been in the host country for more than five years; 62% obtained their education abroad; and 12% arrived in the host country at age 6 or younger, 9% between the ages 7 and 12, and 82% after age 12 (Table B.3.11 in Annex B).
Figure 2.18 compares the performance in literacy of native-born adults of native-born parents to that of foreign-born adults of foreign-born parents with a range of migration-related characteristics. Overall, foreign-born adults of foreign-born parents have higher literacy proficiency if they speak the language of the host country at home (Panel A); if they have been living in the host country for more than five years (Panel B), if their highest educational qualification was obtained in the host country (Panel C); if they arrived in the host country at a young age (Panel D). Doorstep respondents are included in Panel A and Panel B. The share of doorstep respondents among foreign-born of foreign-born parents by language and duration are provided in Table B.3.19 in Annex B.
On average, native-born adults of native-born parents scored 267 points in literacy. Foreign-born adults of foreign-born parents averaged 243 points if they speak the host country’s language at home, but only 204 points if they do not (Panel A). Those who obtained their education in the host country scored 247 points compared to 227 points for those who were educated abroad (Panel B). Looking at the age of arrival, foreign-born adults of foreign-born parents scored 257 points on average if they were aged 6 or younger when they arrived, 247 points if they were between the ages 7 and 12, and 231 points if they were 13 or older (Panel C). Those who have been in the host country for more than five years scored on average 224 points, compared to 214 points among more recent arrivals (Panel D). Results for numeracy and adaptive problem solving are provided in Table A.2.11 in Annex A.
These findings are in line with results from the first cycle of the Survey of Adult Skills and PISA. The first cycle of the Survey of Adult Skills also found notable differences in numeracy and literacy performance, with foreign-born adults experiencing a disadvantage compared to native-born adults.7 It also found that a portion of these gaps can be attributed to factors related to their individual migration histories (OECD, 2019[39]; OECD, 2018[50]). Similarly, the latest PISA report found that the significant gaps in reading and mathematics performance between immigrant and non-immigrant 15-year-old students diminish when accounting for socio-economic background and language spoken at home (OECD, 2023[31]).8
Figure 2.18. Average literacy proficiency, by immigrant background and migration history
Copy link to Figure 2.18. Average literacy proficiency, by immigrant background and migration historyAverage literacy scores
Note: Adults aged 16-65. Panels A and B include adults who were only administered the doorstep interview due to a language barrier (see Box 1.1 in Chapter 1). Panels C and D do not include adults who were only administered the doorstep interview due to a language barrier, as information on country of education and age of arrival was not collected for those respondents (see Box 1.1 in Chapter 1). Foreign-born of foreign-born parents include adults who reported being foreign-born in the doorstep interview, while native-born doorstep respondents are not part of any of the groups presented in this figure. Results for some groups in Chile, Croatia, Czechia, Finland, Hungary, Japan, Korea, Latvia, Lithuania, the Netherlands, Poland, Portugal, and the Slovak Republic are not reported because there are too few observations to provide reliable estimates. *Caution is required in interpreting results due to the high share of respondents with unusual response patterns. See the Note for Poland in the Reader’s Guide.
Countries and economies are ranked in descending order of the average score of native-born adults of native-born parents.
Source: Table A.2.10 (L) and Table A.2.11 (L) in Annex A.
Differences in skills proficiency related to parental education
Differences in academic achievement are associated with differences in socio-economic background. Socio-economically advantaged families have more financial, human and social capital resources. As a consequence, individuals raised in such families are more likely to benefit from access to learning materials, receive parental support for learning, attend better schools and be guided towards activities that will promote their eventual career success (Conger and Donnellan, 2007[51]).
Socio-economic status is a construct that captures various dimensions, such as parents’ income, education and occupational status. This report uses the highest educational qualification obtained by the respondents’ parents as a proxy for their socio-economic background in childhood. This section groups adults into those with highly educated parents (having at least one parent who had attained tertiary education), those with medium-educated parents (having at least one parent who had attained upper secondary education, but none who attained tertiary) and those with low-educated parents (neither parent had attained upper secondary education). As the level of education of parents is not recorded in the doorstep interview, adults who only answered the doorstep interview are excluded from the analysis presented in this section.
Adults with highly educated parents scored higher on average than those with medium-educated parents in the 2023 Survey of Adult Skills, while in turn adults with medium-educated parents outscored those with low-educated parents. This pattern holds across all domains, but the size of the gaps varies across countries. For example, proficiency gaps to the advantage of adults with highly educated parents are relatively high in Germany while relatively low in Spain.
Figure 2.19 shows the average proficiency scores grouped by parents’ attainment level across participating OECD countries and economies for all three domains. Overall, adults with highly educated parents scored 284 points in literacy on average, those with medium-educated parents scored 264 points and those with low-educated parents scored 234 points (Panel A). Results for numeracy and adaptive problem solving are provided in Panels B and C.
Figure 2.20 shows the proficiency gap between adults with highly educated parents and those with low-educated parents. On average, across participating OECD countries and economies, the unadjusted differences are 50 points in literacy (Panel A), 49 points in numeracy (Panel B) and 42 points in adaptive problem solving (Panel C), to the advantage of adults with highly educated parents.
The largest unadjusted gaps are observed in literacy in Germany and Switzerland (70 points and more), in numeracy in Germany and the United States (70 points and more), and in adaptive problem solving in Germany and Switzerland (more than 57 points), all to the advantage of adults with highly educated parents. Conversely, the smallest (yet still significant) gaps, to the advantage of adults with highly educated parents, on average, are observed in literacy in the Slovak Republic, Spain and Sweden (less than 33 points); in numeracy in Lithuania, Spain and Sweden (less than 36 points); and in adaptive problem solving in the Slovak Republic, Spain and Sweden (less than 29 points). Accounting for relevant background factors explains almost half of these gaps.
Generally, the findings are in line with the differences in proficiency observed among adults in the first cycle of the Survey of Adult Skills, which also found that adults whose parents had higher-level qualifications achieved higher average proficiency scores and that background characteristics explained a large part of the estimated gap (OECD, 2016[4]). PISA survey results also found that 15-year-olds with socio-economically advantaged backgrounds have greater proficiency in mathematics (OECD, 2023[31]) and science (OECD, 2016[52]). A study comparing 15-year-olds (PISA) and 27-year-olds (first cycle of the Survey of Adult Skills) has also shown that literacy disparities between students of low- and highly educated parents tend to widen over time, with the gap becoming more pronounced at the bottom end of the proficiency distribution (Borgonovi and Pokropek, 2021[53]).
Figure 2.19. Average proficiency in key information-processing skills, by parental education
Copy link to Figure 2.19. Average proficiency in key information-processing skills, by parental educationLiteracy, numeracy and adaptive problem solving
Note: Adults aged 16-65; does not include adults who were only administered the doorstep interview due to a language barrier, as information on parental education was not collected for those respondents (see Box 1.1 in Chapter 1). Respondents are categorised as having highly educated parents if at least one parent attained tertiary education; as having medium-educated parents if at least one parent attained upper secondary education and none of the parents attained tertiary education; and as having low-educated parents if neither parent attained upper secondary education. *Caution is required in interpreting results due to the high share of respondents with unusual response patterns. See the Note for Poland in the Reader’s Guide.
Countries and economies are ranked in descending order of the average proficiency of adults with low-educated parents.
Source: Table A.2.12 (L, N, A) in Annex A.
Figure 2.20. Differences in key information-processing skills, by parental education
Copy link to Figure 2.20. Differences in key information-processing skills, by parental educationAdjusted and unadjusted differences in average literacy, numeracy and adaptive problem solving scores between adults (highly educated parents minus low-educated parents)
Note: Adults aged 16-65; does not include adults who were only administered the doorstep interview due to a language barrier, as information on parental education was not collected for those respondents (see Box 1.1 in Chapter 1). Respondents are categorised as having highly educated parents if at least one parent attained tertiary education; as having medium-educated parents if at least one parent attained upper secondary education and none of the parents attained tertiary education; and as having low-educated parents if neither parent attained upper secondary education. Unadjusted differences are the differences between the two averages for each contrast category. Adjusted differences are based on a regression model that takes into account differences associated with gender, age, education, immigrant background and language spoken at home. All differences are statistically significant at the 5% level. *Caution is required in interpreting results due to the high share of respondents with unusual response patterns. See the Note for Poland in the Reader’s Guide.
Countries and economies are ranked in descending order of the unadjusted difference between adults with highly educated and low-educated parents. Source: Table A.2.12 (L, N, A) in Annex A.
Table 2.7. Chapter 2 figures and tables
Copy link to Table 2.7. Chapter 2 figures and tables
Table 2.1 |
Comparison of countries and economies based on average proficiency in literacy |
Table 2.2 |
Comparison of countries and economies based on average proficiency in numeracy |
Table 2.3 |
Comparison of countries and economies based on average proficiency in adaptive problem solving |
Table 2.4 |
Description of what adults can do at each proficiency level in literacy |
Table 2.5 |
Description of what adults can do at each proficiency level in numeracy |
Table 2.6 |
Description of what adults can do at each proficiency level in adaptive problem solving |
Figure 2.1 |
Comparison of countries’ and economies’ average proficiency in literacy and numeracy |
Figure 2.2 |
Literacy proficiency among adults |
Figure 2.3 |
Numeracy proficiency among adults |
Figure 2.4 |
Proficiency in adaptive problem solving among adults |
Figure 2.5 |
Share of adults who are low performing in more than one domain |
Figure 2.6 |
Inequality in the distribution of key information-processing skills |
Figure 2.7 |
Average proficiency in key information-processing skills, by age |
Figure 2.8 |
Average proficiency in key information-processing skills, by educational attainment |
Figure 2.9 |
Differences in key information-processing skills, by educational attainment |
Figure 2.10 |
Share of tertiary-educated adults who studied STEM fields |
Figure 2.11 |
Average numeracy proficiency among tertiary-educated adults, by field of study |
Figure 2.12 |
Share of women among STEM and non-STEM graduates |
Figure 2.13 |
Gender differences in numeracy among STEM graduates |
Figure 2.14 |
Gender differences in key information-processing skills |
Figure 2.15 |
Share of low performers in key information-processing skills, by gender |
Figure 2.16 |
Average proficiency in key information-processing skills, by immigrant background |
Figure 2.17 |
Differences in literacy proficiency, by immigrant background |
Figure 2.18 |
Average literacy proficiency, by immigrant background and migration history |
Figure 2.19 |
Average proficiency in key information-processing skills, by parental education |
Figure 2.20 |
Differences in key information-processing skills, by parental education |
References
[9] Baltes, P. (1993), “The aging mind: Potential and limits”, The Gerontologist, Vol. 33/5, pp. 580-594, https://doi.org/10.1093/geront/33.5.580.
[19] Barro, R. and J. Lee (2013), “A new data set of educational attainment in the world, 1950–2010”, Journal of Development Economics, Vol. 104, pp. 184-198, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jdeveco.2012.10.001.
[44] Borgonovi, F., A. Choi and M. Paccagnella (2021), “The evolution of gender gaps in numeracy and literacy between childhood and young adulthood”, Economics of Education Review, Vol. 82, p. 102119, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.econedurev.2021.102119.
[53] Borgonovi, F. and A. Pokropek (2021), “The evolution of socio‐economic disparities in literacy skills from age 15 to age 27 in 20 countries”, British Educational Research Journal, Vol. 47/6, pp. 1560-1586, https://doi.org/10.1002/berj.3738.
[33] Borgonovi, F., H. Seitz and I. Vogel (2022), “Swimming skills around the world: Evidence on inequalities in life skills across and within countries”, OECD Social, Employment and Migration Working Papers, No. 281, OECD Publishing, Paris, https://doi.org/10.1787/0c2c8862-en.
[51] Conger, R. and M. Donnellan (2007), “An interactionist perspective on the socioeconomic context of human development”, Annual Review of Psychology, Vol. 58/1, pp. 175-199, https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085551.
[12] Cunha, F. et al. (2006), “Interpreting the evidence on life cycle skill formation”, in Handbook of the Economics of Education, Elsevier, https://doi.org/10.1016/s1574-0692(06)01012-9.
[46] Cvencek, D., A. Meltzoff and A. Greenwald (2011), “Math-gender stereotypes in elementary school children”, Child Development, Vol. 82/3, pp. 766-779, https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01529.x.
[17] Desjardins, R. (2003), “Determinants of literacy proficiency: A lifelong-lifewide learning perspective”, International Journal of Educational Research, Vol. 39/3, pp. 205-245, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijer.2004.04.004.
[11] Desjardins, R. and A. Warnke (2012), “Ageing and skills: A review and analysis of skill gain and skill loss over the lifespan and over time”, OECD Education Working Papers, No. 72, OECD Publishing, Paris, https://doi.org/10.1787/5k9csvw87ckh-en.
[45] González de San Román, A. and S. De la Rica (2020), “Gender gaps in PISA test scores: The impact of social norms and the mother’s transmission of role attitudes”, Studies of Applied Economics, Vol. 34/1, pp. 79-108, https://doi.org/10.25115/eae.v34i1.3008.
[23] Green, A., F. Green and N. Pensiero (2015), “Cross-country variation in adult skills inequality”, Comparative Education Review, Vol. 59/4, pp. 595-618, https://doi.org/10.1086/683101.
[22] Green, A. and N. Pensiero (2016), “The effects of upper‐secondary education and training systems on skills inequality. A quasi‐cohort analysis using PISA 2000 and the OECD Survey of Adult Skills”, British Educational Research Journal, Vol. 42/5, pp. 756-779, https://doi.org/10.1002/berj.3236.
[14] Grewenig, E. et al. (2021), “COVID-19 and educational inequality: How school closures affect low- and high-achieving students”, European Economic Review, Vol. 140, p. 103920, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.euroecorev.2021.103920.
[20] Hanushek, E. and L. Woessmann (2011), “The economics of international differences in educational achievement”, in Handbook of the Economics of Education, Elsevier, https://doi.org/10.1016/b978-0-444-53429-3.00002-8.
[21] Hanushek, E. and L. Woessmann (2008), “The role of cognitive skills in economic development”, Journal of Economic Literature, Vol. 46/3, pp. 607-668, https://doi.org/10.1257/jel.46.3.607.
[8] Kautz, T. et al. (2014), “Fostering and measuring skills: Improving cognitive and non-cognitive skills to promote lifetime success”, OECD Education Working Papers, No. 110, OECD Publishing, Paris, https://doi.org/10.1787/5jxsr7vr78f7-en.
[30] Klasen, S. (2002), “Low schooling for girls, slower growth for all? Cross-country evidence on the effect of gender inequality in education on economic development”, The World Bank Economic Review, Vol. 16/3, pp. 345-373, https://www.jstor.org/stable/3990191.
[36] Lee, J. and H. Lee (2016), “Human capital in the long run”, Journal of Development Economics, Vol. 122, pp. 147-169, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jdeveco.2016.05.006.
[32] Monticone, C. (2023), “Gender differences in financial literacy and resilience”, in Joining Forces for Gender Equality: What is Holding us Back?, OECD Publishing, Paris, https://doi.org/10.1787/a9f80ab9-en.
[41] OECD (2024), Bridging Talent Shortages in Tech: Skills-first, Micro-credentials and Inclusive Outreach Strategies, Getting Skills Right, OECD Publishing, Paris, https://doi.org/10.1787/f35da44f-en.
[37] OECD (2024), Education at a Glance 2024: OECD Indicators, OECD Publishing, Paris, https://doi.org/10.1787/c00cad36-en.
[26] OECD (2024), PISA 2022 Results (Volume III): Creative Minds, Creative Schools, PISA, OECD Publishing, Paris, https://doi.org/10.1787/765ee8c2-en.
[25] OECD (2024), Social and Emotional Skills for Better Lives: Findings from the OECD Survey on Social and Emotional Skills 2023, OECD Publishing, Paris, https://doi.org/10.1787/35ca7b7c-en.
[5] OECD (2024), Survey of Adult Skills 2023: Reader’s Companion, OECD Skills Studies, OECD Publishing, Paris, https://doi.org/10.1787/3639d1e2-en.
[49] OECD (2023), International Migration Outlook 2023, OECD Publishing, Paris, https://doi.org/10.1787/b0f40584-en.
[40] OECD (2023), Joining Forces for Gender Equality: What is Holding us Back?, OECD Publishing, Paris, https://doi.org/10.1787/67d48024-en.
[31] OECD (2023), PISA 2022 Results (Volume I): The State of Learning and Equity in Education, PISA, OECD Publishing, Paris, https://doi.org/10.1787/53f23881-en.
[34] OECD (2023), Reporting Gender Pay Gaps in OECD Countries: Guidance for Pay Transparency Implementation, Monitoring and Reform, Gender Equality at Work, OECD Publishing, Paris, https://doi.org/10.1787/ea13aa68-en.
[13] OECD (2022), Education at a Glance 2022: OECD Indicators, OECD Publishing, Paris, https://doi.org/10.1787/3197152b-en.
[1] OECD (2021), OECD Skills Outlook 2021: Learning for Life, OECD Publishing, Paris, https://doi.org/10.1787/0ae365b4-en.
[6] OECD (2021), The Assessment Frameworks for Cycle 2 of the Programme for the International Assessment of Adult Competencies, OECD Skills Studies, OECD Publishing, Paris, https://doi.org/10.1787/4bc2342d-en.
[38] OECD (2021), “Why do more young women than men go on to tertiary education?”, Education Indicators in Focus, OECD Publishing, Paris, https://doi.org/10.1787/6f7209d1-en.
[27] OECD (2020), PISA 2018 Results (Volume V): Effective Policies, Successful Schools, PISA, OECD Publishing, Paris, https://doi.org/10.1787/ca768d40-en.
[16] OECD (2020), “The potential of online learning for adults: Early lessons from the COVID-19 crisis”, OECD Policy Responses to Coronavirus (COVID-19), OECD Publishing, Paris, https://doi.org/10.1787/ee040002-en.
[2] OECD (2019), Education Policy Outlook 2019: Working Together to Help Students Achieve their Potential, OECD Publishing, Paris, https://doi.org/10.1787/2b8ad56e-en.
[35] OECD (2019), Fast Forward to Gender Equality: Mainstreaming, Implementation and Leadership, OECD Publishing, Paris, https://doi.org/10.1787/g2g9faa5-en.
[39] OECD (2019), Skills Matter: Additional Results from the Survey of Adult Skills, OECD Skills Studies, OECD Publishing, Paris, https://doi.org/10.1787/1f029d8f-en.
[3] OECD (2018), Equity in Education: Breaking Down Barriers to Social Mobility, PISA, OECD Publishing, Paris, https://doi.org/10.1787/9789264073234-en.
[50] OECD (2018), Skills on the Move: Migrants in the Survey of Adult Skills, OECD Skills Studies, OECD Publishing, Paris, https://doi.org/10.1787/9789264307353-en.
[54] OECD (2017), PISA 2015 Technical Report, OECD, Paris, https://www.oecd.org/content/dam/oecd/en/about/programmes/edu/pisa/publications/technical-report/PISA2015_TechRep_Final.pdf.
[52] OECD (2016), PISA 2015 Results (Volume I): Excellence and Equity in Education, PISA, OECD Publishing, Paris, https://doi.org/10.1787/9789264266490-en.
[4] OECD (2016), Skills Matter: Further Results from the Survey of Adult Skills, OECD Skills Studies, OECD Publishing, Paris, https://doi.org/10.1787/9789264258051-en.
[48] OECD (2015), “Does math make you anxious?”, PISA in Focus, No. 48, OECD Publishing, Paris, https://doi.org/10.1787/5js6b2579tnx-en.
[43] OECD (2015), The ABC of Gender Equality in Education: Aptitude, Behaviour, Confidence, PISA, OECD Publishing, Paris, https://doi.org/10.1787/9789264229945-en.
[47] OECD (2013), “Mathematics self-beliefs and participation in mathematics-related activities”, in PISA 2012 Results: Ready to Learn (Volume III): Students’ Engagement, Drive and Self-Beliefs, OECD Publishing, Paris, https://doi.org/10.1787/9789264201170-8-en.
[18] OECD (2013), OECD Skills Outlook 2013: First Results from the Survey of Adult Skills, OECD Publishing, Paris, https://doi.org/10.1787/9789264204256-en.
[28] OECD (n.d.), Technology Pact, OECD AI Policy Observatory, https://oecd.ai/en/dashboards/policy-initiatives/http:%2F%2Faipo.oecd.org%2F2021-data-policyInitiatives-24780 (accessed on 14 October 2023).
[29] Office of Science and Technology Policy (2021), Progress Report on the Implementation of the Federal STEM Education Strategic Plan, Executive Office of the President of the United States, https://www.whitehouse.gov/wp-content/uploads/2022/01/2021-CoSTEM-Progress-Report-OSTP.pdf.
[7] Paccagnella, M. (2015), “Skills and Wage Inequality: Evidence from PIAAC”, OECD Education Working Papers, No. 114, OECD Publishing, Paris, https://doi.org/10.1787/5js4xfgl4ks0-en.
[24] Silva, P. et al. (2020), “Student selection and performance in higher education: Admission exams vs. high school scores”, Education Economics, Vol. 28/5, pp. 437-454, https://doi.org/10.1080/09645292.2020.1782846.
[10] Toga, A. and P. Thompson (2005), “Genetics of brain stucture and intelligence”, Annual Review of Neuroscience, Vol. 28/1, pp. 1-23, https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.28.061604.135655.
[42] Vallee, C. (2021), Gender bias in AI: What the United States, Canada, and Western European nations can learn from other countries, OECD AI Policy Observatory, https://oecd.ai/en/wonk/ai-national-gender-bias.
[15] Werner, K. and L. Woessmann (2021), “The legacy of COVID-19 in education”, CESifo Working Paper, No. 9358, Ifo Institute Center for Economic Studies, https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.3945280.
Notes
Copy link to Notes← 1. The correlation between the average performance in literacy and adaptive problem solving is 0.86, and between numeracy and adaptive problem solving 0.85.
← 2. A predecessor international skills survey, the Adult Literacy and Life Skills Survey (ALL), found a similar correlation between prose literacy and problem solving, as did the Programme for International Student Assessment (PISA) between reading and mathematics among 15-year-olds (OECD, 2017[54]).
← 3. The Survey of the Adult Skills and PISA measure different constructs, and data in the two studies are collected in different settings. As proficiency from the two studies cannot be directly compared, the analysis presented here is mainly suggestive. For more information on the relationship between the Survey of Adult Skills and PISA, readers are advised to consult Chapter 6 in OECD (2024[5]).
← 4. Permanent-type international migration comprises the following categories: work-related, free movement (e.g. within the EU/EFTA countries, between Australia and New Zealand under the Trans-Tasman Travel Arrangement), accompanying family of workers, family migration and humanitarian migration (OECD, 2023[49]).
← 5. Ukrainians do not fall under the category of permanent-type international migration. In the European Union (EU), the Temporary Protection Directive was activated, leading to a significant number of Ukrainian refugees registering for temporary protection across EU member states. Outside the EU, various countries have developed their own programmes to facilitate the arrival of Ukrainians. For instance, Canada introduced the Canada-Ukraine Authorization for Emergency Travel, the United States established the Uniting for Ukraine programme, and the United Kingdom implemented three parallel schemes: the Ukraine Family Scheme, the Ukraine Extension Scheme, and the Homes for Ukraine Sponsorship Scheme. Alternative legal grounds for staying are also used alongside these pathways (OECD, 2023[49]).
← 6. Only permanent residents are part of the target population of the Survey of Adult Skills. It is possible that in some countries recent migrants who are considered to be temporary residents are excluded from these statistics. See Chapter 5 of OECD (2024[5]) for more information on the sampling frames used in the Survey of Adult Skills.
← 7. Note that past international reports on the results of the Survey of Adult Skills used a different definition of immigrant background. In the first cycle of the Survey of Adult Skills, a strict distinction was made based on the country of birth and only distinguishing between foreign-born and native-born adults (hence disregarding any information about parents’ country of birth).
← 8. PISA 2022 distinguishes between first and second-generation immigrants and defines non-immigrant students as those who have at least one parent born in the country of the assessment.